Knowledge

Wolfgang Prinz

Source 📝

216:. Its core assumption is that actions are coded in terms of the perceivable effects (i.e., the distal perceptual events) they should generate Performing a movement leaves behind a bidirectional association between the motor pattern it has generated by and the sensory effects that it produces. Such an association can then be used backwards to retrieve a movement by anticipating its effects. These perception/action codes are also accessible during action observation (for an historical account of the ideo-motor principle, see Observation of an action should activate action representations to the degree that the perceived and the represented action are similar. Such a claim suggests that we represent observed, executed and imagined actions in a commensurate manner and makes specific predictions regarding the nature of action and perceptual representations. First, representations for observed and executed actions should rely on a shared neural substrate. Second, a common cognitive system predicts interference effects when action and perception attempt to access shared representations simultaneously. Third, such a system predicts facilitation of action based on directly prior perception and vice versa. 31: 243:
in the ventral premotor and posterior parietal cortices fire both during goal-directed actions and observation of the same actions performed by another individual, to functional neuroimaging experiments in humans which indicate that the neural circuits involved in action execution partly overlap with
195:
Academia Europaea; German Academy of Natural Scientist Leopoldina, Halle (Saale), Germany; Scientific Advisory Board of the Centre for Interdisciplinary Research (ZiF), University of Bielefeld, Germany; Advisory Board of the Dean, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Jacobs University Bremen,
265:
Drost, U. C., Rieger, M., Brass, M., Gunter, T. C., & Prinz, W. (2005). "When hearing turns into playing: Movement induction by auditory stimuli in pianists." The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 58A(8),
390:
Sommerville, J. A., & Decety, J. (2006). Weaving the fabric of social interaction: Articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13,
259:
Prinz, W. (2006). "Free will as a social institution." In S. Pockett, W. P. Banks, & S. Gallagher (Eds.), Does consciousness cause behavior? (pp. 257–276). Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
418:
Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., Prinz, W., & Wascher, E. (2006). Twin Peaks: An ERP study of action planning and control in co-acting individuals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 859-870.
354:
Hommel, B., Müsseler, Aschersleben, G. and Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849-937.
187:
for Psychological Research Munich, Germany, from 1990 to 2004. Since 2004 he has been a director at the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany.
427:
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and the imitation of action. Nature Review Neuroscience, 2, 661-670.
400:
Brass, M., Schmitt, R., Spengler, S. & Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: inferential processes versus motor simulation. Current Biology 17, 24, 2117-2121.
262:
Bosbach, S., Cole, J., Prinz, W., & Knoblich, G. (2005). "Inferring another's expectation from action: The role of peripheral sensation." Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 1295–1297.
132: 345:
Prinz, W. (2003). Experimental approaches to action. In J. Roessler & N. Eilan (Eds.). Agency and Self-awareness (pp. 175-187). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
498: 256:
Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Prinz, W. (2007). "Perceptual resonance: Action-induced modulation of perception." Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 349–355.
272:
Prinz, W., Beisert, M., & Herwig, A. (Eds.). (2013). Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
483: 381:
Knoblich, G. & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of actions: interactions of perception and action. Psychological Science, 12, 467-472.
409:
Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The social nature of perception and action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 99-104.
253:
Meltzoff, A. & Prinz, W. (2002). "The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution and Brain Bases." Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
436:
Decety, J., & Grèzes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Research, 1079, 4-14.
508: 196:
Germany; Honorary Member of the European Society of Psychology (ESCoP); Psychonomic Society; German Society of Psychology (DGPs).
503: 363:
Hommel, B. (2004). Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 494-500.
239:. In neuroscience, evidence for the common coding theory ranges from electrophysiological recordings in monkeys in which 468: 478: 269:
Prinz, W. (2012). Open minds: The social making of agency and intentionality. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
488: 372:
Stock, A. & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of the ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68, 176-188.
155:
between perception and action that has a significant impact in cognitive neuroscience and social cognition.
336:
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129-154.
220: 176: 69: 282: 140: 473: 224: 219:
The common coding theory has received strong support from a variety of empirical studies in
493: 205: 184: 152: 144: 115: 105: 80: 8: 213: 148: 131:(born 24 September 1942) is a German cognitive psychologist. He is the director of the 228: 325: 287: 183:
from the Ruhr University in Bochum, Germany, in 1970. Prinz was a director of the
297: 292: 236: 232: 240: 462: 454: 307: 209: 168: 164: 302: 180: 190: 172: 136: 90:
Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz Award of the German Research Foundation
59: 48: 30: 44: 133:
Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences
139:, Germany, and an internationally recognized expert in 449: 235:. This theory is at the core of what has been called 326:Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Prinz. Max-Planck-Gesellschaft. 460: 179:(Germany) from 1962 to 1966, and was awarded a 191:Memberships in Research Councils and Societies 499:Corresponding fellows of the British Academy 244:those activated when actions are observed. 29: 199: 484:Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz Prize winners 461: 208:. This theory claims parity between 13: 14: 520: 442: 247: 430: 421: 412: 403: 504:Max Planck Institute directors 394: 384: 375: 366: 357: 348: 339: 330: 319: 1: 313: 158: 16:German cognitive psychologist 509:University of Münster alumni 7: 276: 204:Prinz is the father of the 151:. He is the founder of the 10: 525: 469:Max Planck Society people 122: 111: 101: 94: 86: 75: 65: 55: 37: 28: 21: 453:Wolfgang Prinz web page 221:developmental psychology 479:Cognitive psychologists 283:Experimental psychology 163:Wolfgang Prinz studied 141:experimental psychology 489:German neuroscientists 225:cognitive neuroscience 448:Max-Planck Institute 200:Academic achievements 177:University of Münster 118:(professor, director) 70:University of Münster 206:common coding theory 185:Max Planck Institute 153:common coding theory 145:cognitive psychology 116:Max Planck Institute 106:Cognitive Psychology 81:common coding theory 149:philosophy of mind 229:cognitive science 126: 125: 96:Scientific career 41:24 September 1942 516: 437: 434: 428: 425: 419: 416: 410: 407: 401: 398: 392: 388: 382: 379: 373: 370: 364: 361: 355: 352: 346: 343: 337: 334: 328: 323: 288:Social cognition 33: 19: 18: 524: 523: 519: 518: 517: 515: 514: 513: 459: 458: 445: 440: 435: 431: 426: 422: 417: 413: 408: 404: 399: 395: 389: 385: 380: 376: 371: 367: 362: 358: 353: 349: 344: 340: 335: 331: 324: 320: 316: 298:Sense of agency 293:Motor cognition 279: 250: 237:Motor cognition 233:neurophysiology 202: 193: 161: 66:Alma mater 51: 42: 24: 17: 12: 11: 5: 522: 512: 511: 506: 501: 496: 491: 486: 481: 476: 471: 457: 456: 451: 444: 443:External links 441: 439: 438: 429: 420: 411: 402: 393: 383: 374: 365: 356: 347: 338: 329: 317: 315: 312: 311: 310: 305: 300: 295: 290: 285: 278: 275: 274: 273: 270: 267: 263: 260: 257: 254: 249: 248:Selected works 246: 241:mirror neurons 201: 198: 192: 189: 160: 157: 129:Wolfgang Prinz 124: 123: 120: 119: 113: 109: 108: 103: 99: 98: 92: 91: 88: 84: 83: 77: 76:Known for 73: 72: 67: 63: 62: 57: 53: 52: 43: 39: 35: 34: 26: 25: 23:Wolfgang Prinz 22: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 521: 510: 507: 505: 502: 500: 497: 495: 492: 490: 487: 485: 482: 480: 477: 475: 474:Living people 472: 470: 467: 466: 464: 455: 452: 450: 447: 446: 433: 424: 415: 406: 397: 387: 378: 369: 360: 351: 342: 333: 327: 322: 318: 309: 306: 304: 301: 299: 296: 294: 291: 289: 286: 284: 281: 280: 271: 268: 264: 261: 258: 255: 252: 251: 245: 242: 238: 234: 230: 226: 222: 217: 215: 211: 207: 197: 188: 186: 182: 178: 174: 170: 166: 156: 154: 150: 146: 142: 138: 134: 130: 121: 117: 114: 110: 107: 104: 100: 97: 93: 89: 85: 82: 78: 74: 71: 68: 64: 61: 58: 54: 50: 46: 40: 36: 32: 27: 20: 432: 423: 414: 405: 396: 386: 377: 368: 359: 350: 341: 332: 321: 218: 203: 194: 162: 128: 127: 112:Institutions 95: 494:1942 births 56:Nationality 463:Categories 314:References 308:Philosophy 266:1376–1389. 210:perception 169:Philosophy 165:Psychology 159:Background 303:Imitation 181:doctorate 391:179-200. 277:See also 175:at the 173:Zoology 137:Leipzig 49:Bavaria 214:action 102:Fields 87:Awards 60:German 45:Ebern 231:and 212:and 171:and 147:and 79:The 38:Born 135:in 465:: 227:, 223:, 167:, 143:, 47:,

Index


Ebern
Bavaria
German
University of Münster
common coding theory
Cognitive Psychology
Max Planck Institute
Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences
Leipzig
experimental psychology
cognitive psychology
philosophy of mind
common coding theory
Psychology
Philosophy
Zoology
University of Münster
doctorate
Max Planck Institute
common coding theory
perception
action
developmental psychology
cognitive neuroscience
cognitive science
neurophysiology
Motor cognition
mirror neurons
Experimental psychology

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.