Knowledge

N2pc

Source đź“ť

101:
controlled processes of directing attention. The same study also demonstrated that the N2pc does not only occur when attending to visual features, but semantic features as well. In one experiment, subjects had to respond to the words "left" and "right" while ignoring the color words "white" and "brown." Even in this case of semantically defined targets, subjects demonstrated an N2pc contralateral to the target word. Together, these results have provided strong evidence that the N2pc reflects the location of covert, consciously directed attention.
97:
items (e.g., a green square among blue squares). The oddball stimuli would "pop out" and attract attention, but were not necessarily targets. As a result, experimenters knew where subjects directed attention, but could simultaneously manipulate factors orthogonal to the location of attention, such as low-level features or probability of the target appearing. The pop-out oddball would generate an N2pc, as it received focused attention, while stimulus characteristics modulated the amplitude and latency of the component.
105:
should be identical in all conditions, and the experimenter should only manipulate instructions for directing attention across conditions; this precludes the possibility that stimulus features drive ERP effects rather than focused attention. Second, the target should be easy to find, usually via "pop-out." The goal is to minimize the variability in search times and N2pc latency, resulting in a much clearer signal when the waveforms are averaged together over multiple trials.
81:
contralateral to the attended side of the visual field relative to ipsilateral electrode sites. For example, when a person pays attention to something in the left side of the visual field, an N2pc appears as a greater negativity over the right posterior areas of the brain than the left. MEG has been used to localize the N2pc primarily to lateral extrastriate cortex and inferotemporal visual areas, such as V4.
59:. Subjects viewed arrays containing 4-12 items, one of which was a target on 50% of trials. Compared to the waveform over cortex ipsilateral to the target, experimenters observed a consistently greater negative deflection of the ERP waveform at approximately 200 ms after the stimulus at posterior sites (i.e., over visual cortex) contralateral to the side of the screen subjects attended. 173:
to be tied to a spatial filtering hypothesis (see above: "Eliminating the N2pc"). The last point regarding the functional significance of the N2pc, however, has been challenged. Some have contested the spatial filtering hypothesis, arguing that the N2pc reflects an enhancement of task-relevant stimulus processing rather than a suppression of irrelevant stimuli.
109:
told to attend to either the red T's or the green T's at the beginning of the experiment and report whether that letter is upright or inverted (thus fulfilling the second criterion that attention should not be confounded with stimulus characteristics). We should expect to see an N2pc contralateral to the side of the screen the attended letter appeared.
138:
In a dual-task situation where subjects focus on a demanding primary task while performing target detection as a secondary task, the N2pc only appears in response to detecting targets defined by conjunctions of features. Again, the greater attentional demands of conjunction-based targets relative to
104:
The prototypical visual search paradigm for eliciting an N2pc component has subjects attend and respond to a target stimulus to the left or right of fixation. Unlike regular visual search experiments, however, two major criteria most hold when attempting to measure N2pc response. First, the stimuli
176:
Other work has explored further cognitive processes that could be linked to the N2pc. For instance, the classic visual search paradigm that elicits the N2pc could be broken down further into processes of shifting attention, and spatially based processing of non-target locations. When combining the
100:
Subsequent investigation into the N2pc manipulated the number of items in the array and found that a display with as few as two objects elicits the component. Because an object cannot "pop out" and attract attention in a two-item display, experimenters concluded that the N2pc must reflect top-down,
172:
The N2pc literature agrees on a few functional characteristics of the N2pc. First, the N2pc appears whenever a person focuses attention on an object. Second, it serves as a direct measure for the direction of focused attention, either to the left or right. Finally, the N2pc is generally believed
108:
An example experiment for eliciting the N2pc that follows the critical principles above might proceed as follows: Subjects see an array of upright and inverted T's. One T is red, and one T is green, but the rest are black (thus fulfilling the first criterion of easy-to-find targets). Subjects are
96:
The first experiments to investigate specifically the N2pc used a visual search paradigm in which subjects reported the presence or absence of a pre-defined target (e.g., a green rectangle) in a display containing one "oddball" stimulus that differed on a single feature from a uniform background of
67:
paradigm in which subjects had to report the presence of a target object in a display (e.g., a green box or a horizontal bar). They confirmed that the N2pc appeared contralateral to attended stimuli, and furthermore found that it did not appear when subjects saw only one object at a time or had to
163:
The N2pc also disappears when targets in the visual search task are defined as "any oddball object" rather than by one or more specific features. Luck and Hillyard (1994) have argued that in this case, determining whether a given object is a target requires distributing attention over multiple
80:
ERP components, a negative deflection in the ERP waveform at a latency of approximately 200-300 ms following a stimulus. The "pc" stands for "posterior-contralateral", describing the topographic distribution of the component. It appears as a greater negativity at posterior electrode sites
62:
The N2pc first received its name from Luck and Hillyard (1994), who named the component after its characteristic features. The "N" denotes a negative polarity; "2" describes its latency in the waveform (i.e., the second negative deflection, typically around 200 ms); and "pc" stands for
159:
Early investigations of the N2pc critically found that the component was sensitive to the presence of distractors, appearing only when distractors accompanied a target stimulus. Furthermore, N2pc amplitude increases with the number of distractors in the display.
177:
visual search task with visual cues that drew attention to spatial locations in the display, experimenters found that while the N2pc may not reflect shifts of attention, it may still reflect processing of a location in space that may or may not contain a target.
122:
The N2pc is primarily sensitive to the directional focus of attention over time. However, research has found a variety of factors that modulate N2pc response. N2pc amplitude is sensitive to factors related to increasing demands on focused attention.
155:
Certain experimental conditions can eliminate the N2pc entirely. These results have been used to argue for a spatial filtering hypothesis, which proposes that the N2pc reflects the process of ignoring task-irrelevant (i.e., non-target) stimuli.
68:
spread their attention over all the items in the display. These data led the experimenters to believe the N2pc corresponds to a filtering process that occurs whenever people focus attention on one object while ignoring others.
127:
When non-target stimuli closely resemble the target (e.g., when targets are defined by size, but the size difference between targets and distractors is very small), they elicit an N2pc of lower amplitude than a target
142:
N2pc amplitude increases as distractors appear closer to the target, which increases the need to focus on the target while filtering the distractors (but also see Mazza et al., 2009, who found conflicting
63:"posterior-contralateral," as the component appears over posterior electrode sites contralateral to the direction of attention. The experimenters explored what factors would modulate the N2pc using a 500:
Luck, S.J.; Girelli, M.; McDermott, M.T.; Ford, M.A. (1997). "Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention".
89:
The N2pc can be used flexibly in nearly any task in which one would like to study the direction and time course of selective attention. However, researchers have primarily used the N2pc in
164:
objects in the array (and determining the common features) rather than filtering them. Consequently, the spatial filtering process is discouraged, and the N2pc therefore does not appear.
312:
Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Electrophysiological evidence for parallel and serial processing during visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, 603-617.
135:(e.g., blue, horizontal bar) rather than a single feature (e.g., blue bar), they elicit a larger N2pc, which may reflect a greater demand on attention to identify the target. 39:, and vice versa. This characteristic makes it a useful tool for directly measuring the general direction of a person's attention (either left or right) with fine-grained 146:
When subjects have to indicate where a target is located, they exhibit a larger N2pc than when they simply have to report whether or not a target is present
299:
Luck, S. J. (2005). "The operation of attention—millisecond by millisecond—over the first half second." In H. Ogmen & B. G. Breitmeyer (Eds.),
759: 782: 93:
paradigms to study the deployment of attention over time and test hypotheses of parallel and serial models of visual search.
1013: 752: 31:
contralateral to the location in space to which subjects are attending; if subjects pay attention to the left side of the
807: 425:
Luck, S. J.; Hillyard, S. A. (1994). "Spatial filtering during visual search: Evidence from human electrophysiology".
328:
Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search.
1034: 745: 222: 833: 802: 272: 207: 868: 197: 301:
The first half second: The microgenesis and temporal dynamics of unconscious and conscious visual processes.
1039: 650:
Mazza, V.; Turatto, M.; Caramazza, A. (2009). "Attention selection, distractor suppression, and N2pc".
342:
Hopf, J.-M.; Luck, S. J.; Girelli, M.; Hagner, T.; Mangun, G. R.; Scheich, H.; Heinze, H. J. (2000).
76:
The component's name, N2pc, abbreviates its characteristics. The component belongs to the family of
397: 998: 883: 466: 212: 202: 192: 56: 20: 919: 787: 217: 695:"The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing" 132: 828: 797: 768: 461: 392: 187: 792: 599: 383:
Woodman, G. F.; Luck, S. J. (2003). "Serial deployment of attention during visual search".
8: 972: 913: 898: 873: 863: 853: 267: 262: 242: 227: 77: 40: 603: 993: 929: 888: 719: 694: 675: 568: 525: 724: 710: 667: 627: 622: 587: 560: 556: 517: 479: 475: 365: 52: 679: 529: 1003: 817: 714: 706: 659: 617: 607: 552: 509: 471: 434: 402: 355: 572: 663: 543:
Treisman, A.M.; Gelade, G. (1980). "A feature-integration theory of attention".
438: 360: 343: 737: 406: 1028: 967: 90: 64: 28: 51:
Luck and Hillyard (1990) first observed the N2pc while seeking to document
957: 728: 671: 513: 369: 32: 631: 612: 564: 521: 483: 452:
Eimer, M. (1996). "The N2pc as an indicator of attentional selectivity".
952: 947: 937: 962: 942: 843: 277: 24: 427:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
385:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
977: 1008: 36: 893: 848: 838: 257: 237: 232: 55:
correlates of focused attention during visual search using
908: 903: 588:"On the role of selective attention in visual perception" 252: 247: 499: 344:"Neural sources of focused attention in visual search" 649: 341: 592:
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA
692: 454:
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology
783:Amplitude integrated electroencephalography (aEEG) 35:, the N2pc appears in the right hemisphere of the 84: 1026: 767: 542: 753: 693:Kiss, M.; Van Velzen, J.; Eimer, M. (2008). 495: 493: 424: 71: 382: 1014:Neurophysiological Biomarker Toolbox (NBT) 760: 746: 645: 643: 641: 295: 293: 167: 718: 621: 611: 490: 465: 396: 359: 139:feature-based targets may be responsible. 112: 585: 638: 420: 418: 416: 324: 322: 320: 318: 290: 1027: 445: 741: 451: 413: 315: 869:Contingent negative variation (CNV) 808:Brainstem auditory evoked potential 13: 14: 1051: 586:Luck, S. J.; Ford, M. A. (1998). 711:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00611.x 686: 579: 223:Lateralized readiness potential 803:Somatosensory evoked potential 536: 376: 335: 306: 273:Somatosensory evoked potential 208:Early left anterior negativity 150: 131:When targets are defined by a 85:Classic paradigms and findings 23:component linked to selective 1: 999:Difference due to memory (Dm) 284: 198:Contingent negative variation 798:Magnetoencephalography (MEG) 769:Electroencephalography (EEG) 664:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.10.009 557:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5 476:10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9 117: 7: 793:Electrocorticography (ECoG) 439:10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000 180: 10: 1056: 407:10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.121 46: 986: 928: 816: 775: 361:10.1093/cercor/10.12.1233 72:Component characteristics 27:. The N2pc appears over 303:Cambridge, MA: MIT Press 213:Error-related negativity 203:Difference due to memory 920:Late positive component 788:Event-related potential 218:Late positive component 168:Functional significance 133:conjunction of features 1035:Electroencephalography 829:Bereitschaftspotential 514:10.1006/cogp.1997.0660 188:Bereitschaftspotential 113:Functional sensitivity 613:10.1073/pnas.95.3.825 545:Cognitive Psychology 502:Cognitive Psychology 330:Psychophysiology 31, 53:electrophysiological 973:Sensorimotor rhythm 930:Neural oscillations 874:Mismatch negativity 604:1998PNAS...95..825L 263:P300 (neuroscience) 228:Mismatch negativity 41:temporal resolution 1040:Evoked potentials 1022: 1021: 916:(late positivity) 818:Evoked potentials 354:(12): 1233–1241. 1047: 1004:Oddball paradigm 762: 755: 748: 739: 738: 733: 732: 722: 699:Psychophysiology 690: 684: 683: 647: 636: 635: 625: 615: 583: 577: 576: 540: 534: 533: 497: 488: 487: 469: 449: 443: 442: 433:(5): 1000–1014. 422: 411: 410: 400: 380: 374: 373: 363: 339: 333: 326: 313: 310: 304: 297: 1055: 1054: 1050: 1049: 1048: 1046: 1045: 1044: 1025: 1024: 1023: 1018: 982: 924: 812: 771: 766: 736: 691: 687: 648: 639: 584: 580: 541: 537: 498: 491: 450: 446: 423: 414: 398:10.1.1.457.3225 381: 377: 348:Cerebral Cortex 340: 336: 327: 316: 311: 307: 298: 291: 287: 282: 183: 170: 153: 120: 115: 87: 74: 49: 12: 11: 5: 1053: 1043: 1042: 1037: 1020: 1019: 1017: 1016: 1011: 1006: 1001: 996: 990: 988: 984: 983: 981: 980: 975: 970: 965: 960: 955: 950: 945: 940: 934: 932: 926: 925: 923: 922: 917: 911: 906: 901: 896: 891: 886: 881: 877: 876: 871: 866: 861: 856: 851: 846: 841: 836: 831: 826: 822: 820: 814: 813: 811: 810: 805: 800: 795: 790: 785: 779: 777: 773: 772: 765: 764: 757: 750: 742: 735: 734: 705:(2): 240–249. 685: 658:(7): 879–890. 637: 598:(3): 825–830. 578: 535: 489: 467:10.1.1.545.141 460:(3): 225–234. 444: 412: 375: 334: 314: 305: 288: 286: 283: 281: 280: 275: 270: 265: 260: 255: 250: 245: 240: 235: 230: 225: 220: 215: 210: 205: 200: 195: 190: 184: 182: 179: 169: 166: 152: 149: 148: 147: 144: 140: 136: 129: 119: 116: 114: 111: 86: 83: 73: 70: 48: 45: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1052: 1041: 1038: 1036: 1033: 1032: 1030: 1015: 1012: 1010: 1007: 1005: 1002: 1000: 997: 995: 992: 991: 989: 985: 979: 976: 974: 971: 969: 968:Sleep spindle 966: 964: 961: 959: 956: 954: 951: 949: 946: 944: 941: 939: 936: 935: 933: 931: 927: 921: 918: 915: 912: 910: 907: 905: 902: 900: 897: 895: 892: 890: 887: 885: 882: 879: 878: 875: 872: 870: 867: 865: 862: 860: 857: 855: 852: 850: 847: 845: 842: 840: 837: 835: 832: 830: 827: 824: 823: 821: 819: 815: 809: 806: 804: 801: 799: 796: 794: 791: 789: 786: 784: 781: 780: 778: 776:Related tests 774: 770: 763: 758: 756: 751: 749: 744: 743: 740: 730: 726: 721: 716: 712: 708: 704: 700: 696: 689: 681: 677: 673: 669: 665: 661: 657: 653: 646: 644: 642: 633: 629: 624: 619: 614: 609: 605: 601: 597: 593: 589: 582: 574: 570: 566: 562: 558: 554: 551:(1): 97–136. 550: 546: 539: 531: 527: 523: 519: 515: 511: 507: 503: 496: 494: 485: 481: 477: 473: 468: 463: 459: 455: 448: 440: 436: 432: 428: 421: 419: 417: 408: 404: 399: 394: 390: 386: 379: 371: 367: 362: 357: 353: 349: 345: 338: 331: 325: 323: 321: 319: 309: 302: 296: 294: 289: 279: 276: 274: 271: 269: 266: 264: 261: 259: 256: 254: 251: 249: 246: 244: 241: 239: 236: 234: 231: 229: 226: 224: 221: 219: 216: 214: 211: 209: 206: 204: 201: 199: 196: 194: 191: 189: 186: 185: 178: 174: 165: 161: 157: 145: 141: 137: 134: 130: 126: 125: 124: 110: 106: 102: 98: 94: 92: 91:visual search 82: 79: 69: 66: 65:visual search 60: 58: 54: 44: 42: 38: 34: 30: 29:visual cortex 26: 22: 19:refers to an 18: 994:10-20 system 958:Theta rhythm 858: 702: 698: 688: 655: 651: 595: 591: 581: 548: 544: 538: 508:(1): 64–87. 505: 501: 457: 453: 447: 430: 426: 388: 384: 378: 351: 347: 337: 329: 308: 300: 175: 171: 162: 158: 154: 121: 107: 103: 99: 95: 88: 75: 61: 50: 33:visual field 16: 15: 884:C1 & P1 391:: 121–138. 151:Elimination 1029:Categories 953:Delta wave 948:Gamma wave 938:Alpha wave 880:Positivity 825:Negativity 285:References 963:K-complex 943:Beta wave 844:Visual N1 462:CiteSeerX 393:CiteSeerX 278:Visual N1 193:C1 and P1 143:results). 118:Amplitude 25:attention 729:17971061 680:10575482 672:19084218 530:11065167 370:11073872 332:291-308. 181:See also 128:stimulus 978:Mu wave 720:2248220 632:9448247 600:Bibcode 565:7351125 522:9212722 484:8862112 47:History 1009:EEGLAB 987:Topics 727:  717:  678:  670:  652:Cortex 630:  620:  573:353246 571:  563:  528:  520:  482:  464:  395:  368:  676:S2CID 623:33804 569:S2CID 526:S2CID 37:brain 914:P600 899:P300 894:P200 864:N400 859:N2pc 854:N200 849:N170 839:N100 834:ELAN 725:PMID 668:PMID 628:PMID 561:PMID 518:PMID 480:PMID 366:PMID 268:P600 258:P200 243:N400 238:N170 233:N100 57:ERPs 17:N2pc 909:P3b 904:P3a 889:P50 715:PMC 707:doi 660:doi 618:PMC 608:doi 553:doi 510:doi 472:doi 435:doi 403:doi 356:doi 253:P3b 248:P3a 21:ERP 1031:: 723:. 713:. 703:45 701:. 697:. 674:. 666:. 656:45 654:. 640:^ 626:. 616:. 606:. 596:95 594:. 590:. 567:. 559:. 549:12 547:. 524:. 516:. 506:33 504:. 492:^ 478:. 470:. 458:99 456:. 431:20 429:. 415:^ 401:. 389:29 387:. 364:. 352:10 350:. 346:. 317:^ 292:^ 78:N2 43:. 761:e 754:t 747:v 731:. 709:: 682:. 662:: 634:. 610:: 602:: 575:. 555:: 532:. 512:: 486:. 474:: 441:. 437:: 409:. 405:: 372:. 358::

Index

ERP
attention
visual cortex
visual field
brain
temporal resolution
electrophysiological
ERPs
visual search
N2
visual search
conjunction of features
Bereitschaftspotential
C1 and P1
Contingent negative variation
Difference due to memory
Early left anterior negativity
Error-related negativity
Late positive component
Lateralized readiness potential
Mismatch negativity
N100
N170
N400
P3a
P3b
P200
P300 (neuroscience)
P600
Somatosensory evoked potential

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑