Knowledge

Prototype theory

Source đź“ť

738:
if you look at them you will not see something common to all, but similarities, relationships, and a whole series of them at that. To repeat: don't think, but look! Look for example at board games, with their multifarious relationships. Now pass to card games; here you find many correspondences with the first group, but many common features drop out, and others appear. When we pass next to ball games, much that is common is retained, but much is lost. Are they all 'amusing'? Compare chess with noughts and crosses. Or is there always winning and losing, or competition between players? Think of patience. In ball games there is winning and losing; but when a child throws his ball at the wall and catches it again, this feature has disappeared. Look at the parts played by skill and luck; and at the difference between skill in chess and skill in tennis. Think now of games like ring-a-ring-a-roses; here is the element of amusement, but how many other characteristic features have disappeared! And we can go through the many, many other groups of games in the same way; can see how similarities crop up and disappear. And the result of this examination is: we see a complicated network of similarities overlapping and criss-crossing: sometimes overall similarities, sometimes similarities of detail.
942:
are warm and cuddly but fish cannot be. Fish are often eaten for dinner, but pets are never. Hence the conjunctive prototype fails to inherit features of either concept that are incompatible with the other concept. A final stage in the process looks for knowledge of the class in long term memory, and if the class is familiar may add extra features - a process called "extensional feedback". The model was tested by showing how apparently logical syntactic conjunctions or disjunctions, such as "A sport which is also a game" or "Vehicles that are not Machines", or "Fruits or Vegetables" fail to conform to Boolean set logic. Chess is considered to be a sport which is a game, but is not considered to be a sport. Mushrooms are considered to be either a fruit or a vegetable, but when asked separately very few people consider them to be a vegetable and no-one considers them to be a fruit.
580:. These features may or may not be true of all members of the class (necessary or defining features), but they will all be associated with being a typical member or the class. By this means, two aspects of concept structure can be explained. Some exemplars are more typical of a category than others, because they are a better fit to the concept prototype, having more of the features. Importantly, Hampton's prototype model explains the vagueness that can occur at the boundary of conceptual categories. While some may think of pictures, telephones or cookers as atypical furniture, others will say they are not furniture at all. Membership of a category can be a matter of degree, and the same features that give rise to typicality structure are also responsible for graded degrees of category membership. 902:, have suggested problems with the prototype theory. In their 1999 paper, they raise several issues. One of which is that prototype theory does not intrinsically guarantee graded categorization. When subjects were asked to rank how well certain members exemplify the category, they rated some members above others. For example, robins were seen as being "birdier" than ostriches, but when asked whether these categories are "all-or-nothing" or have fuzzier boundaries, the subjects stated that they were defined, "all-or-nothing" categories. Laurence and Margolis concluded that "prototype structure has no implication for whether subjects represent a category as being graded" (p. 33). 557:
conditions are those that no other entity possesses. Rather than defining concepts by features, the prototype theory defines categories based on either a specific artifact of that category or by a set of entities within the category that represent a prototypical member. The prototype of a category can be understood in lay terms by the object or member of a class most often associated with that class. The prototype is the center of the class, with all other members moving progressively further from the prototype, which leads to the gradation of categories. Every member of the class is not equally central in human cognition. As in the example of
911: 25: 713:
classified based on similarity to stored prototypes and stored exemplars, whereas participants who only had experience with exemplar only relied on the similarity to stored exemplars. Smith and Minda looked at the use of prototypes and exemplars in dot-pattern category learning. They found that participants used more prototypes than they used exemplars, with the prototypes being the center of the category, and exemplars surrounding it.
612:— a chair is associated with bending of one's knees, a fruit with picking it up and putting it in your mouth, etc. At the subordinate level (e.g. , etc.) few significant features can be added to that of the basic level; whereas at the superordinate level, these conceptual similarities are hard to pinpoint. A picture of a chair is easy to draw (or visualize), but drawing furniture would be more difficult. 941:
James Hampton found that prototypes for conjunctive concepts such as pet fish are produced by a compositional function operating on the features of each concept. Initially all features of each concept are added to the prototype of the conjunction. There is then a consistency check - for example pets
737:
Consider for example the proceedings that we call 'games'. I mean board games, card games, ball games, Olympic games, and so on. What is common to them all? Don't say, "There must be something common, or they would not be called 'games'"--but look and see whether there is anything common to all. For
712:
A common comparison is the use of prototype or the use of exemplars in category classification. Medin, Altom, and Murphy found that using a mixture of prototype and exemplar information, participants were more accurately able to judge categories. Participants who were presented with prototype values
945:
Antonio Lieto and Gian Luca Pozzato have proposed a typicality-based compositional logic (TCL) that is able to account for both complex human-like concept combinations (like the PET-FISH problem) and conceptual blending. Their framework shows how concepts expressed as prototypes can account for the
615:
Psychologists Eleanor Rosch, Carolyn Mervis and colleagues defined the basic level as that level that has the highest degree of cue validity and category validity. Thus, a category like may have a prototypical member, but no cognitive visual representation. On the other hand, basic categories in ,
850:
Mikulincer, Mario & Paz, Dov & Kedem, Perry focused on the dynamic nature of prototypes and how represented semantic categories actually changes due to emotional states. The 4 part study assessed the relationships between situational stress and trait anxiety and the way people organize the
747:
describes the phenomenon when people group concepts based on a series of overlapping features, rather than by one feature which exists throughout all members of the category. For example, basketball and baseball share the use of a ball, and baseball and chess share the feature of a winner, etc.,
697:
Subsequent to Rosch's work, prototype effects have been investigated widely in areas such as colour cognition, and also for more abstract notions: subjects may be asked, e.g. "to what degree is this narrative an instance of telling a lie?". Similar work has been done on actions (verbs like look,
556:
Rosch and others developed prototype theory as a response to, and radical departure from, the classical theory of concepts, which defines concepts by necessary and sufficient conditions. Necessary conditions refers to the set of features every instance of a concept must present, and sufficient
639:
However, the notion of Basic-ness as a Level can be problematic. Linguistically, types of bird (swallow, robin, gull) are basic level - they have mono-morphemic nouns, which fall under the superordinate BIRD, and have subordinates expressed by noun phrases (herring gull, male robin). Yet in
553:'s study "Natural Categories", was initially defined as denoting a stimulus, which takes a salient position in the formation of a category, due to the fact that it is the first stimulus to be associated with that category. Rosch later defined it as the most central member of a category. 810:), one encounters the question of whether or not the prototype of is a 6 foot tall man, or a 400-foot skyscraper. The solution emerges by contextualizing the notion of prototype in terms of the object being modified. This extends even more radically in compounds such as 654:
While one may differ from this list in terms of cultural specifics, the point is that such a graded categorization is likely to be present in all cultures. Further evidence that some members of a category are more privileged than others came from experiments involving:
2069: 851:
hierarchical level at which semantic stimuli are categorized, the way people categorize natural objects, the narrowing of the breadth of categories and the proneness to use less inclusive levels of categorization instead of more inclusive ones.
640:
psychological terms, bird behaves as a basic level term. At the same time, atypical birds such as ostrich and penguin are themselves basic level terms, having very distinct outlines and not sharing obvious parts with other birds.
623:
Clearly semantic models based on attribute-value pairs fail to identify privileged levels in the hierarchy. Functionally, it is thought that basic level categories are a decomposition of the world into maximally
643:
More problems arise when the notion of a prototype is applied to lexical categories other than the noun. Verbs, for example, seem to defy a clear prototype: is hard to split up in more or less central members.
569:. Contrary to the classical view, prototypes and gradations lead to an understanding of category membership not as an all-or-nothing approach, but as more of a web of interlocking categories which overlap. 748:
rather than one defining feature of "games". Therefore, there is a distance between focal, or prototypical members of the category, and those that continue outwards from them, linked by shared features.
1967:
Gatsgeb, H.Z., Dundas, E.M., Minshew, M.J., & Strauss, M.S. (2012). Category formation in autism: Can individuals with autism form categories and prototypes of dot patterns?.
1852:
Lieto, Antonio; Pozzato, Gian Luca (2020). "A description logic framework for commonsense conceptual combination integrating typicality, probabilities and cognitive heuristics".
1448:
Medin, Douglas L.; Altom, Mark W.; Murphy, Timothy D. (1984). "Given versus induced category representations: Use of prototype and exemplar information in classification".
458:, in which there is a graded degree of belonging to a conceptual category, and some members are more central than others. It emerged in 1971 with the work of psychologist 914:
A guppy is not a prototype pet, nor a prototype fish, but it is a prototype pet-fish. This challenges the idea that prototypes are created from their constituent parts.
759:, where a category is defined in terms of a conceptual distance. More central members of a category are "between" the peripheral members. He postulates that most 647:
In her 1975 paper, Rosch asked 200 American college students to rate, on a scale of 1 to 7, whether they regarded certain items as good examples of the category
1937:
Dirven, R. & Taylor, J.R. (1988): "The conceptualisation of vertical Space in English: The Case of Tall", pp. 379-402 in Brygida Rudzka-Ostyn (ed.),
651:. These items ranged from chair and sofa, ranked number 1, to a love seat (number 10), to a lamp (number 31), all the way to a telephone, ranked number 60. 882:
Douglas L. Medin and Marguerite M. Schaffer showed by experiment that a context theory of classification which derives concepts purely from exemplars (cf.
2080: 1918: 2111:
Mikulincer, Mario & Kedem-Friedrich, Peri & Paz, Dov. (1990). Anxiety and categorization—1. The structure and boundaries of mental categories.
2097:
Mikulincer, Mario, Kedem, Peri & Paz, Dov (1990), "The impact of trait anxiety and situational stress on the categorization of natural objects",
2182:, E., "Classification of Real-World Objects: Origins and Representations in Cognition", pp. 212–222 in Johnson-Laird, P.N. & Wason, P.C., 2032: 674:: When primed with the higher-level (superordinate) category, subjects were faster in identifying if two words are the same. Thus, after flashing 1233: 2148: 2129:
Mikulincer, Mario & Paz, Dov & Kedem-Friedrich, Peri. (1990). Anxiety and categorization—2. Hierarchical levels of mental categories.
725:'s (later) discomfort with the traditional notion of category. This influential theory has resulted in a view of semantic components more as 488:
in any given language has a real world example that best represents this concept. For example: when asked to give an example of the concept
1825:
Hampton, James A. (1988). "Overextension of conjunctive concepts: Evidence for a unitary model of concept typicality and class inclusion".
1082:
Rosch, Eleanor; Mervis, Carolyn B; Gray, Wayne D; Johnson, David M; Boyes-Braem, Penny (July 1976). "Basic objects in natural categories".
822:
in the prototypical sense, but the red indicates merely a shift from the prototypical colour of wine or hair respectively. The addition of
521:
In formulating prototype theory, Rosch drew in part from previous insights in particular the formulation of a category model based on
2049:
A description logic framework for commonsense conceptual combination integrating typicality, probabilities and cognitive heuristics
1930: 327: 2290: 572:
Further development of prototype theory by psychologist James Hampton, and others replaced the notion of prototypes being the
2042: 2023: 1946: 1271: 1185: 230: 430: 2070:
Spreading Activation Within Semantic Categories: Comments on Rosch’s “Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories”
1547: 2232:, E., "Prototype Classification and Logical Classification: The Two Systems", pp. 73–86 in Scholnick, E.K. (ed), 755:
has elaborated a possible partial explanation of prototype theory in terms of multi-dimensional feature spaces called
2333: 1647: 1572: 1432: 1218: 990: 225: 68: 46: 39: 1951: 1329:
Collier, George A.; Berlin, Brent; Kay, Paul (March 1973). "Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution".
2374: 2359: 362: 162: 806:
These combinations pose a lesser problem in terms of prototype theory. In situations involving adjectives (e.g.
2369: 2162: 763:
categories exhibit a convexity in conceptual space, in that if x and y are elements of a category, and if z is
94: 2002: 1739:; Lepore, Ernest (February 1996). "The red herring and the pet fish: why concepts still can't be prototypes". 791:
is to be defined as the set of objects having this property. This does not apply as well to modifiers such as
2011: 1685:
Osherson, Daniel N.; Smith, Edward E. (1981). "On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts".
980: 2323: 301: 2081:
Given versus induced category representations: Use of prototype and exemplar information in classification
2364: 2282:, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P., "Basic Objects in Natural Categories", 899: 834:
The prototype is changed by additional specific information, and combines features from the prototype of
702: 470:. It has been criticized by those that still endorse the traditional theory of categories, like linguist 934:
might be trout or salmon. However, the features of these prototypes do not present in the prototype for
2349: 1671: 530: 955: 2048: 1096: 620:
and semantic features. Basic level categories tend to have the same parts and recognizable images.
608:
in cognitive categorization. Basic categories are relatively homogeneous in terms of sensory-motor
220: 99: 33: 1211:
Categories, prototypes and exemplars, in Routledge Handbook of Semantics, Ed. N.Riemer, pp.125-141
2262:, E. & Mervis, C.B., "Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal Structure of Categories", 995: 467: 423: 384: 379: 121: 1596:
Medin, Douglas L.; Schaffer, Marguerite M. (1978). "Context theory of classification learning".
1091: 860: 347: 203: 172: 50: 938:, therefore this prototype must be generated from something other than its constituent parts. 784: 588: 584: 463: 455: 357: 342: 140: 2291:
Distinguishing prototype-based and exemplar-based processes in dot-pattern category learning
2222:, E., "Principles of Categorization", pp. 27–48 in Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B.B. (eds), 1871: 1000: 868: 729:
rather than necessary contributors to the meaning of texts. His discussion on the category
475: 247: 145: 693:: When asked to name a few exemplars, the more prototypical items came up more frequently. 8: 2318: 1415:
Geeraerts, Dirk; Dirven, René; Taylor, John R.; Langacker, Ronald W., eds. (2001-01-31).
910: 722: 332: 267: 130: 1982: 1875: 752: 2354: 1887: 1861: 1772: 1718: 1665: 1621: 1397: 1354: 1277: 1263: 1158: 1109: 970: 960: 744: 625: 616:
i.e. , , , are full of informational content and can easily be categorized in terms of
522: 416: 2329: 2142: 2124: 2038: 2019: 1942: 1807: 1764: 1756: 1752: 1710: 1702: 1698: 1653: 1643: 1613: 1578: 1568: 1543: 1520: 1512: 1473: 1465: 1428: 1389: 1372:
Coleman, Linda; Kay, Paul (March 1981). "Prototype Semantics: The English Word Lie".
1346: 1311: 1267: 1214: 1191: 1181: 1150: 1145: 1128: 1105: 1064: 1060: 451: 337: 257: 126: 2276:, Language Behaviour Research Laboratory, University of California (Berkeley), 1975. 1891: 1776: 1722: 1625: 1281: 2302: 2170: 2138: 2120: 2102: 2088: 2065: 2056: 1990: 1972: 1958: 1879: 1834: 1799: 1748: 1694: 1605: 1504: 1492: 1457: 1420: 1381: 1338: 1303: 1259: 1140: 1113: 1101: 1056: 895: 756: 322: 262: 252: 193: 167: 2060: 1883: 1925: 965: 883: 864: 471: 2306: 1508: 1307: 591:
also have a prototype structure, like categories of common words in a language.
2092: 1790:
Hampton, James A. (1987). "Inheritance of attributes in concept conjunctions".
1642:. Margolis, Eric, 1968-, Laurence, Stephen. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. 1999. 1609: 1461: 985: 447: 352: 272: 185: 2174: 2106: 1994: 1976: 1838: 2343: 2279: 2269: 2259: 2249: 2239: 2229: 2219: 2209: 2199: 2189: 2179: 2028: 1760: 1706: 1617: 1582: 1516: 1469: 1393: 1350: 1315: 1195: 1154: 1068: 550: 459: 285: 1657: 1495:(2005). "Category Representation for Classification and Feature Inference". 1251: 1981:
Gatsgeb, H.Z., Wilkinson, D.A., Minshew, M.J., & Strauss, M.S. (2011).
1909: 1524: 1294:
Rosch, Eleanor (1975). "Cognitive representations of semantic categories".
706: 526: 280: 198: 16:
Theory of categorization based upon degrees of similarity to a central case
1811: 1768: 1714: 1477: 1424: 1736: 886:) worked better than a class of theories that included prototype theory. 507: 466:" in the theory of categorization for its departure from the traditional 404: 2034:
Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind
632:
maximize the number of attributes shared by members of the category, and
2234:
New Trends in Conceptual Representation: Challenges to Piaget’s Theory?
1803: 1162: 975: 617: 609: 576:
exemplar, with the proposal that a prototype is a bundle of correlated
134: 1401: 1358: 1954:
Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland
859:
Prototype theory has been criticized by those that still endorse the
515: 511: 490: 84: 2163:
When prototypes are not best: Judgments made by children with autism
1962: 946:
phenomenon of prototypical compositionality in concept combination.
2295:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
2085:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
1913: 1866: 1827:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
1497:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
1450:
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition
1385: 1342: 871: 667:) elicited faster response times than for non-prototypical members. 502: 478: 399: 2272:, E., Mervis, C.B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., & Boyes-Braem, P., 775:
Within language we find instances of combined categories, such as
701:
Another aspect in which Prototype Theory departs from traditional
1983:
Can individuals with autism abstract prototypes of natural faces?
485: 2053:
Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence
1854:
Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence
1565:
Geometry of meaning : semantics based on conceptual spaces
1129:"On the Prototype Theory of Concepts and the Definition of Art" 1414: 635:
minimize the number of attributes shared with other categories
2246:, Vol.6, Nos.11-12, (November/December 1999), pp. 61–77. 2226:, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers, (Hillsdale), 1978. 2149:
The Prototype Effect in Recognition Memory: Intact in Autism?
923: 496: 2161:
Molesworth, C.J., Bowler, D.M., & Hampton, J.A. (2008).
2147:
Molesworth, C.J., Bowler, D.M., & Hampton, J.A. (2005).
2202:, E., "Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories", 1238:
Linguistic Categorization: Prototypes in Linguistic Theory
767:
x and y, then z is also likely to belong to the category.
2274:
Basic Objects in Natural Categories, Working Paper No.43
663:: in which queries involving prototypical members (e.g. 709:
categories (bird, dog) vs. artifacts (toys, vehicles).
1081: 1047:
Rosch, Eleanor H. (1973-05-01). "Natural categories".
2079:
Medin, D.L., Altom, M.W., & Murphy, T.D. (1984).
926:
kept in a bowl in someone's house. The prototype for
918:
Daniel Osherson and Edward Smith raised the issue of
2256:, Vol.104, No.3, (September 1975), pp. 241–243. 2206:, Vol.104, No.3, (September 1975), pp. 192–233. 1417:
Applied Cognitive Linguistics, II, Language Pedagogy
604:
The other notion related to prototypes is that of a
2076:, Vol.104, No.3, (September 1975), p. 234-240. 1919:
Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution
1678: 698:kill, speak, walk ), adjectives like "tall", etc. 1589: 1208: 845: 2341: 2266:, Vol.7, No.4, (October 1975), pp. 573–605. 2186:, Cambridge University Press, (Cambridge), 1977. 1941:, Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. 1490: 1447: 1328: 2236:, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, 1983. 1175: 1931:Structural Semantics and 'Cognitive' Semantics 2286:, Vol.8, No.3, (July 1976), pp. 382–439. 1999: 1684: 1595: 1562: 1020: 1018: 1016: 930:might be a dog or cat, and the prototype for 721: The notion of prototypes is related to 424: 1851: 1537: 1227: 716: 506:. Prototype theory has also been applied in 2254:Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2204:Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 2192:, E. (1975): “Cognitive Reference Points”, 2167:Journal of Autism and Development Disorders 2074:Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1987:Journal of Autism and Development Disorders 1969:Journal of Autism and Development Disorders 1735: 1296:Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 1133:The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism 1027: 540: 2152:Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2018:(pp. 176-179). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 2003:Conceptual Spaces: The Geometry of Thought 1371: 1013: 431: 417: 1928:, Willems, K. & Leuschner, T. (2000) 1865: 1144: 1095: 783:. Combining categories was a problem for 599: 69:Learn how and when to remove this message 2289:Smith, J.D., & Minda, J.P. (2002). " 2036:, Chicago, IL, Chicago University Press. 1952:Galton, F. (1878). Composite portraits. 909: 889: 787:, where the semantics of a word such as 32:This article includes a list of general 2184:Thinking: Readings in Cognitive Science 1824: 1789: 1126: 770: 2342: 2131:Personality and Individual Differences 2113:Personality and Individual Differences 500:is more frequently cited than, say, a 2212:, E.H. (1973): "Natural categories", 2016:MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science 1293: 1249: 1046: 826:shifts the prototype from the one of 905: 484:In this prototype theory, any given 18: 922:for which the prototype might be a 13: 2047:Lieto, A., Pozzato, G.L. (2019): " 1264:10.1016/b978-1-4832-1446-7.50028-5 877: 705:is that there do not appear to be 462:, and it has been described as a " 38:it lacks sufficient corresponding 14: 2386: 991:Semantic feature-comparison model 454:, particularly in psychology and 2244:Journal of Consciousness Studies 1146:10.1111/j.0021-8529.2005.00203.x 23: 2326:(Philosophische Untersuchungen) 1939:Topics in Cognitive Linguistics 1845: 1818: 1783: 1729: 1632: 1556: 1531: 1484: 1441: 1408: 1365: 1322: 1287: 2328:, Blackwell Publishers, 2001. 1258:, Elsevier, pp. 312–322, 1252:"Principles of Categorization" 1243: 1202: 1169: 1120: 1075: 1040: 846:Dynamic structure and distance 682:is detected more rapidly than 510:, as part of the mapping from 1: 2242:, E., "Reclaiming Concepts", 2061:10.1080/0952813X.2019.1672799 1902: 1884:10.1080/0952813X.2019.1672799 1640:Concepts : core readings 1538:Wittgenstein, Ludwig (1953). 1256:Readings in Cognitive Science 594: 549:, as defined in psychologist 2324:Philosophical Investigations 2224:Cognition and Categorization 2143:10.1016/0191-8869(90)90190-3 2125:10.1016/0191-8869(90)90189-X 1753:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00694-x 1699:10.1016/0010-0277(81)90013-5 1567:. Cambridge, Massachusetts. 1540:Philosophical Investigations 1106:10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-X 1061:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90017-0 1006: 867:and other proponents of the 861:classic theory of categories 535:How shall a thing be called? 474:and other proponents of the 7: 2307:10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1433 1509:10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1433 1308:10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.192 949: 854: 703:Aristotelian categorization 10: 2391: 2315:, Oxford University Press. 2093:10.1037/0278-7393.10.3.333 1610:10.1037/0033-295X.85.3.207 1462:10.1037/0278-7393.10.3.333 1209:Hampton, James A. (2016). 363:Predicate transformational 2313:Linguistic Categorization 2252:, E., "Reply to Loftus", 2175:10.1007/s10803-008-0557-7 2107:10.1080/08917779008249328 1995:10.1007/s10803-011-1190-4 1977:10.1007/s10803-011-1411-x 1839:10.1037/0278-7393.14.1.12 1178:Linguistic categorization 799:is very different from a 743:Wittgenstein's theory of 733:is particularly incisive: 717:Distance between concepts 1542:. Blackwell Publishing. 1176:Taylor, John R. (2009). 1127:Adajian, Thomas (2005). 587:it has been argued that 541:Overview and terminology 2010:Hampton, J.A. (1999). " 2000:Gärdenfors, P. (2004): 1934:, in Logos and Language 1250:Rosch, Eleanor (1988), 1033:Croft and Cruse (2004) 996:Similarity (philosophy) 628:categories. Thus, they 468:Aristotelian categories 385:Abstract semantic graph 380:Abstract interpretation 53:more precise citations. 2375:Philosophy of language 2360:Psychological theories 1792:Memory & Cognition 1670:: CS1 maint: others ( 1180:. Oxford Univ. Press. 915: 741: 600:Basic level categories 512:phonological structure 204:Theory of descriptions 173:Context (language use) 2370:Cognitive linguistics 1989:, 41(12), 1609–1618. 1425:10.1515/9783110866254 1035:Cognitive Linguistics 956:Composite photography 913: 894:Linguists, including 890:Graded categorization 785:extensional semantics 735: 678:, the equivalence of 589:linguistic categories 585:Cognitive linguistics 565:is more central than 464:Copernican Revolution 456:cognitive linguistics 304:programming languages 2311:Taylor, J.R.(2003): 2284:Cognitive Psychology 2264:Cognitive Psychology 2214:Cognitive Psychology 2194:Cognitive Psychology 2169:, 38(9), 1721–1730. 1971:, 42(8), 1694–1704. 1956:, Vol.8, pp.132–142. 1598:Psychological Review 1240:, 2nd ed., ch.2 p.21 1084:Cognitive Psychology 1049:Cognitive Psychology 1001:Intuitive statistics 869:structural semantics 771:Combining categories 476:structural semantics 248:Semantic file system 1876:2020JETAI..32..769L 1563:Gärdenfors, Peter. 1491:Johansen, Mark K.; 268:Semantic similarity 2365:Semantic relations 2087:, 10(3), 333–352. 1804:10.3758/BF03197712 971:Family resemblance 961:Composite portrait 916: 745:family resemblance 523:family resemblance 2350:Cognitive science 2043:978-0-2264-6804-4 2024:978-0-2622-3200-5 1947:978-9-0272-3544-2 1493:Kruschke, John K. 1273:978-1-4832-1446-7 1187:978-0-19-926664-7 906:Compound concepts 818:which are hardly 757:conceptual spaces 665:is a robin a bird 452:cognitive science 441: 440: 392: 391: 296: 295: 258:Semantic matching 79: 78: 71: 2382: 2319:Wittgenstein, L. 2301:(4), 1433–1458. 2099:Anxiety Research 2007: 1896: 1895: 1869: 1849: 1843: 1842: 1822: 1816: 1815: 1787: 1781: 1780: 1733: 1727: 1726: 1682: 1676: 1675: 1669: 1661: 1636: 1630: 1629: 1593: 1587: 1586: 1560: 1554: 1553: 1535: 1529: 1528: 1503:(6): 1433–1458. 1488: 1482: 1481: 1445: 1439: 1438: 1412: 1406: 1405: 1369: 1363: 1362: 1326: 1320: 1319: 1291: 1285: 1284: 1247: 1241: 1231: 1225: 1224: 1206: 1200: 1199: 1173: 1167: 1166: 1148: 1124: 1118: 1117: 1099: 1079: 1073: 1072: 1044: 1038: 1031: 1025: 1022: 896:Stephen Laurence 863:, like linguist 753:Peter Gärdenfors 444:Prototype theory 433: 426: 419: 311: 310: 263:Semantic parsing 253:Semantic desktop 231:Machine-learning 194:Semantic feature 181:Prototype theory 168:Compositionality 110: 109: 81: 80: 74: 67: 63: 60: 54: 49:this article by 40:inline citations 27: 26: 19: 2390: 2389: 2385: 2384: 2383: 2381: 2380: 2379: 2340: 2339: 2338: 2101:, 2:2, 85–101. 1963:10.2307/2841021 1905: 1900: 1899: 1850: 1846: 1823: 1819: 1788: 1784: 1734: 1730: 1683: 1679: 1663: 1662: 1650: 1638: 1637: 1633: 1594: 1590: 1575: 1561: 1557: 1550: 1536: 1532: 1489: 1485: 1446: 1442: 1435: 1413: 1409: 1370: 1366: 1327: 1323: 1292: 1288: 1274: 1248: 1244: 1232: 1228: 1221: 1207: 1203: 1188: 1174: 1170: 1125: 1121: 1097:10.1.1.149.3392 1080: 1076: 1045: 1041: 1032: 1028: 1023: 1014: 1009: 981:Frame semantics 966:Exemplar theory 952: 908: 892: 884:exemplar theory 880: 878:Exemplar theory 865:Eugenio Coseriu 857: 848: 773: 739: 719: 602: 597: 543: 529:(1953), and by 472:Eugenio Coseriu 446:is a theory of 437: 367: 303: 290: 235: 190: 150: 104: 75: 64: 58: 55: 45:Please help to 44: 28: 24: 17: 12: 11: 5: 2388: 2378: 2377: 2372: 2367: 2362: 2357: 2352: 2337: 2336: 2316: 2309: 2287: 2277: 2267: 2257: 2247: 2237: 2227: 2217: 2207: 2197: 2187: 2177: 2159: 2145: 2127: 2109: 2095: 2077: 2063: 2045: 2026: 2008: 1997: 1979: 1965: 1949: 1935: 1923: 1906: 1904: 1901: 1898: 1897: 1860:(5): 769–804. 1844: 1817: 1782: 1747:(2): 253–270. 1728: 1677: 1648: 1631: 1604:(3): 207–238. 1588: 1573: 1555: 1549:978-1405159289 1548: 1530: 1483: 1456:(3): 333–352. 1440: 1433: 1407: 1386:10.2307/414285 1364: 1343:10.2307/412128 1321: 1302:(3): 192–233. 1286: 1272: 1242: 1226: 1219: 1201: 1186: 1168: 1139:(3): 231–236. 1119: 1090:(3): 382–439. 1074: 1055:(3): 328–350. 1039: 1026: 1024:CoČ™eriu (2000) 1011: 1010: 1008: 1005: 1004: 1003: 998: 993: 988: 986:Platonic ideal 983: 978: 973: 968: 963: 958: 951: 948: 907: 904: 891: 888: 879: 876: 856: 853: 847: 844: 801:small elephant 781:small elephant 772: 769: 718: 715: 695: 694: 687: 668: 661:Response Times 637: 636: 633: 601: 598: 596: 593: 542: 539: 448:categorization 439: 438: 436: 435: 428: 421: 413: 410: 409: 408: 407: 402: 394: 393: 390: 389: 388: 387: 382: 374: 373: 369: 368: 366: 365: 360: 355: 350: 345: 340: 335: 330: 325: 319: 316: 315: 307: 306: 298: 297: 294: 293: 292: 291: 289: 288: 283: 277: 275: 273:Semantic query 270: 265: 260: 255: 250: 242: 241: 237: 236: 234: 233: 228: 223: 217: 214: 213: 209: 208: 207: 206: 201: 196: 191: 189: 188: 186:Force dynamics 183: 177: 175: 170: 165: 157: 156: 152: 151: 149: 148: 143: 138: 124: 118: 115: 114: 106: 105: 103: 102: 97: 91: 88: 87: 77: 76: 31: 29: 22: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 2387: 2376: 2373: 2371: 2368: 2366: 2363: 2361: 2358: 2356: 2353: 2351: 2348: 2347: 2345: 2335: 2334:0-631-23127-7 2331: 2327: 2325: 2320: 2317: 2314: 2310: 2308: 2304: 2300: 2296: 2292: 2288: 2285: 2281: 2278: 2275: 2271: 2268: 2265: 2261: 2258: 2255: 2251: 2248: 2245: 2241: 2238: 2235: 2231: 2228: 2225: 2221: 2218: 2215: 2211: 2208: 2205: 2201: 2198: 2195: 2191: 2188: 2185: 2181: 2178: 2176: 2172: 2168: 2164: 2160: 2157: 2153: 2150: 2146: 2144: 2140: 2136: 2132: 2128: 2126: 2122: 2118: 2114: 2110: 2108: 2104: 2100: 2096: 2094: 2090: 2086: 2082: 2078: 2075: 2071: 2067: 2064: 2062: 2058: 2054: 2050: 2046: 2044: 2040: 2037: 2035: 2030: 2027: 2025: 2021: 2017: 2013: 2009: 2005: 2004: 1998: 1996: 1992: 1988: 1984: 1980: 1978: 1974: 1970: 1966: 1964: 1960: 1957: 1955: 1950: 1948: 1944: 1940: 1936: 1933: 1932: 1927: 1924: 1921: 1920: 1915: 1911: 1908: 1907: 1893: 1889: 1885: 1881: 1877: 1873: 1868: 1863: 1859: 1855: 1848: 1840: 1836: 1832: 1828: 1821: 1813: 1809: 1805: 1801: 1797: 1793: 1786: 1778: 1774: 1770: 1766: 1762: 1758: 1754: 1750: 1746: 1742: 1738: 1732: 1724: 1720: 1716: 1712: 1708: 1704: 1700: 1696: 1692: 1688: 1681: 1673: 1667: 1659: 1655: 1651: 1649:0-262-13353-9 1645: 1641: 1635: 1627: 1623: 1619: 1615: 1611: 1607: 1603: 1599: 1592: 1584: 1580: 1576: 1574:0-262-31958-6 1570: 1566: 1559: 1551: 1545: 1541: 1534: 1526: 1522: 1518: 1514: 1510: 1506: 1502: 1498: 1494: 1487: 1479: 1475: 1471: 1467: 1463: 1459: 1455: 1451: 1444: 1436: 1434:9783110866254 1430: 1426: 1422: 1418: 1411: 1403: 1399: 1395: 1391: 1387: 1383: 1379: 1375: 1368: 1360: 1356: 1352: 1348: 1344: 1340: 1336: 1332: 1325: 1317: 1313: 1309: 1305: 1301: 1297: 1290: 1283: 1279: 1275: 1269: 1265: 1261: 1257: 1253: 1246: 1239: 1235: 1234:John R Taylor 1230: 1222: 1220:9780367867591 1216: 1213:. Routledge. 1212: 1205: 1197: 1193: 1189: 1183: 1179: 1172: 1164: 1160: 1156: 1152: 1147: 1142: 1138: 1134: 1130: 1123: 1115: 1111: 1107: 1103: 1098: 1093: 1089: 1085: 1078: 1070: 1066: 1062: 1058: 1054: 1050: 1043: 1037:ch.4 pp.74-77 1036: 1030: 1021: 1019: 1017: 1012: 1002: 999: 997: 994: 992: 989: 987: 984: 982: 979: 977: 974: 972: 969: 967: 964: 962: 959: 957: 954: 953: 947: 943: 939: 937: 933: 929: 925: 921: 912: 903: 901: 900:Eric Margolis 898:writing with 897: 887: 885: 875: 873: 870: 866: 862: 852: 843: 841: 837: 833: 829: 825: 821: 817: 813: 809: 804: 802: 798: 794: 790: 786: 782: 778: 768: 766: 762: 758: 754: 749: 746: 740: 734: 732: 728: 724: 714: 710: 708: 704: 699: 692: 688: 685: 681: 677: 673: 669: 666: 662: 658: 657: 656: 652: 650: 645: 641: 634: 631: 630: 629: 627: 621: 619: 613: 611: 607: 592: 590: 586: 581: 579: 575: 570: 568: 564: 560: 554: 552: 551:Eleanor Rosch 548: 538: 536: 532: 528: 524: 519: 517: 513: 509: 505: 504: 499: 498: 493: 492: 487: 482: 480: 477: 473: 469: 465: 461: 460:Eleanor Rosch 457: 453: 449: 445: 434: 429: 427: 422: 420: 415: 414: 412: 411: 406: 403: 401: 398: 397: 396: 395: 386: 383: 381: 378: 377: 376: 375: 371: 370: 364: 361: 359: 356: 354: 351: 349: 346: 344: 341: 339: 336: 334: 331: 329: 326: 324: 321: 320: 318: 317: 313: 312: 309: 308: 305: 300: 299: 287: 286:Semantic wiki 284: 282: 279: 278: 276: 274: 271: 269: 266: 264: 261: 259: 256: 254: 251: 249: 246: 245: 244: 243: 239: 238: 232: 229: 227: 226:Computational 224: 222: 219: 218: 216: 215: 211: 210: 205: 202: 200: 197: 195: 192: 187: 184: 182: 179: 178: 176: 174: 171: 169: 166: 164: 161: 160: 159: 158: 154: 153: 147: 144: 142: 139: 136: 132: 128: 125: 123: 122:Computational 120: 119: 117: 116: 112: 111: 108: 107: 101: 98: 96: 93: 92: 90: 89: 86: 83: 82: 73: 70: 62: 52: 48: 42: 41: 35: 30: 21: 20: 2322: 2312: 2298: 2294: 2283: 2273: 2263: 2253: 2243: 2233: 2223: 2213: 2203: 2193: 2183: 2166: 2155: 2151: 2134: 2130: 2116: 2112: 2098: 2084: 2073: 2052: 2033: 2015: 2006:, MIT Press. 2001: 1986: 1968: 1953: 1938: 1929: 1917: 1857: 1853: 1847: 1830: 1826: 1820: 1798:(1): 55–71. 1795: 1791: 1785: 1744: 1740: 1737:Fodor, Jerry 1731: 1693:(1): 35–58. 1690: 1686: 1680: 1639: 1634: 1601: 1597: 1591: 1564: 1558: 1539: 1533: 1500: 1496: 1486: 1453: 1449: 1443: 1416: 1410: 1377: 1373: 1367: 1334: 1330: 1324: 1299: 1295: 1289: 1255: 1245: 1237: 1229: 1210: 1204: 1177: 1171: 1136: 1132: 1122: 1087: 1083: 1077: 1052: 1048: 1042: 1034: 1029: 944: 940: 935: 931: 927: 919: 917: 893: 881: 858: 849: 839: 835: 831: 827: 823: 819: 815: 811: 807: 805: 800: 796: 792: 788: 780: 776: 774: 764: 760: 750: 742: 736: 730: 726: 723:Wittgenstein 720: 711: 707:natural kind 700: 696: 690: 683: 679: 675: 671: 664: 660: 653: 648: 646: 642: 638: 622: 614: 605: 603: 582: 577: 574:most typical 573: 571: 566: 562: 558: 555: 546: 544: 534: 527:Wittgenstein 520: 501: 495: 489: 483: 443: 442: 348:Denotational 302:Semantics of 281:Semantic Web 240:Applications 199:Semantic gap 180: 65: 56: 37: 2216:4, 328–350. 2196:7, 532–547. 2137:, 815–821. 2119:, 805–814. 1926:Coseriu, E. 1922:, Berkeley. 830:to that of 797:small mouse 684:stove-stove 680:chair-chair 626:informative 610:affordances 606:basic level 531:Roger Brown 508:linguistics 405:Linguistics 358:Operational 343:Concurrency 338:Categorical 141:Statistical 59:August 2015 51:introducing 2344:Categories 2158:, 496-497. 2029:Lakoff, G. 1910:Berlin, B. 1903:References 1867:1811.02366 1337:(1): 245. 976:Folksonomy 751:Recently, 595:Categories 146:Structural 135:lexicology 95:Linguistic 34:references 2355:Semantics 2068:, E.F., " 1761:0010-0277 1741:Cognition 1707:0010-0277 1687:Cognition 1666:cite book 1618:0033-295X 1583:881289030 1517:1939-1285 1470:1939-1285 1394:0097-8507 1380:(1): 26. 1351:0097-8507 1316:0096-3445 1196:553516096 1155:0021-8529 1092:CiteSeerX 1069:0010-0285 1007:Footnotes 832:red hair. 691:Exemplars 676:furniture 649:furniture 559:furniture 547:prototype 545:The term 516:semantics 491:furniture 333:Axiomatic 328:Algebraic 113:Subfields 85:Semantics 2031:(1987), 2012:Concepts 1916:(1969): 1892:53224988 1833:: 2–32. 1777:15356470 1723:10482356 1658:40256159 1626:27148249 1525:16393056 1374:Language 1331:Language 1282:15633758 950:See also 936:pet fish 920:pet fish 872:paradigm 855:Critique 816:red hair 812:red wine 777:tall man 727:possible 578:features 567:wardrobe 537:(1958). 503:wardrobe 479:paradigm 400:Language 212:Analysis 163:Analysis 1914:Kay, P. 1872:Bibcode 1812:3821491 1769:8820389 1715:7196818 1478:6235306 1236:(1995) 1163:3700527 1114:5612467 765:between 761:natural 672:Priming 618:Gestalt 561:above, 486:concept 127:Lexical 100:Logical 47:improve 2332:  2066:Loftus 2041:  2022:  1945:  1912:& 1890:  1810:  1775:  1767:  1759:  1721:  1713:  1705:  1656:  1646:  1624:  1616:  1581:  1571:  1546:  1523:  1515:  1476:  1468:  1431:  1402:414285 1400:  1392:  1359:412128 1357:  1349:  1314:  1280:  1270:  1217:  1194:  1184:  1161:  1153:  1112:  1094:  1067:  372:Theory 323:Action 221:Latent 155:Topics 36:, but 2280:Rosch 2270:Rosch 2260:Rosch 2250:Rosch 2240:Rosch 2230:Rosch 2220:Rosch 2210:Rosch 2200:Rosch 2190:Rosch 2180:Rosch 1888:S2CID 1862:arXiv 1773:S2CID 1719:S2CID 1622:S2CID 1398:JSTOR 1355:JSTOR 1278:S2CID 1159:JSTOR 1110:S2CID 924:guppy 793:small 563:couch 497:couch 314:Types 131:lexis 2330:ISBN 2039:ISBN 2020:ISBN 1943:ISBN 1808:PMID 1765:PMID 1757:ISSN 1711:PMID 1703:ISSN 1672:link 1654:OCLC 1644:ISBN 1614:ISSN 1579:OCLC 1569:ISBN 1544:ISBN 1521:PMID 1513:ISSN 1474:PMID 1466:ISSN 1429:ISBN 1390:ISSN 1347:ISSN 1312:ISSN 1268:ISBN 1215:ISBN 1192:OCLC 1182:ISBN 1151:ISSN 1065:ISSN 932:fish 840:wine 838:and 828:hair 808:tall 795:; a 731:game 494:, a 353:Game 2303:doi 2293:", 2171:doi 2139:doi 2121:doi 2103:doi 2089:doi 2072:", 2057:doi 2051:", 2014:". 1991:doi 1973:doi 1959:doi 1880:doi 1835:doi 1800:doi 1749:doi 1695:doi 1606:doi 1505:doi 1458:doi 1421:doi 1382:doi 1339:doi 1304:doi 1300:104 1260:doi 1141:doi 1102:doi 1057:doi 928:pet 836:red 824:red 820:red 814:or 789:red 779:or 689:3. 670:2. 659:1. 583:In 533:'s 525:by 514:to 450:in 2346:: 2321:, 2299:28 2297:, 2165:. 2156:46 2154:, 2135:11 2133:, 2117:11 2115:. 2083:. 2055:. 1985:. 1886:. 1878:. 1870:. 1858:32 1856:. 1831:14 1829:. 1806:. 1796:15 1794:. 1771:. 1763:. 1755:. 1745:58 1743:. 1717:. 1709:. 1701:. 1689:. 1668:}} 1664:{{ 1652:. 1620:. 1612:. 1602:85 1600:. 1577:. 1519:. 1511:. 1501:31 1499:. 1472:. 1464:. 1454:10 1452:. 1427:. 1419:. 1396:. 1388:. 1378:57 1376:. 1353:. 1345:. 1335:49 1333:. 1310:. 1298:. 1276:, 1266:, 1254:, 1190:. 1157:. 1149:. 1137:63 1135:. 1131:. 1108:. 1100:. 1086:. 1063:. 1051:. 1015:^ 874:. 842:. 803:. 518:. 481:. 133:, 2305:: 2173:: 2141:: 2123:: 2105:: 2091:: 2059:: 1993:: 1975:: 1961:: 1894:. 1882:: 1874:: 1864:: 1841:. 1837:: 1814:. 1802:: 1779:. 1751:: 1725:. 1697:: 1691:9 1674:) 1660:. 1628:. 1608:: 1585:. 1552:. 1527:. 1507:: 1480:. 1460:: 1437:. 1423:: 1404:. 1384:: 1361:. 1341:: 1318:. 1306:: 1262:: 1223:. 1198:. 1165:. 1143:: 1116:. 1104:: 1088:8 1071:. 1059:: 1053:4 686:. 432:e 425:t 418:v 137:) 129:( 72:) 66:( 61:) 57:( 43:.

Index

references
inline citations
improve
introducing
Learn how and when to remove this message
Semantics
Linguistic
Logical
Computational
Lexical
lexis
lexicology
Statistical
Structural
Analysis
Compositionality
Context (language use)
Prototype theory
Force dynamics
Semantic feature
Semantic gap
Theory of descriptions
Latent
Computational
Machine-learning
Semantic file system
Semantic desktop
Semantic matching
Semantic parsing
Semantic similarity

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑