Knowledge

Oswald Külpe

Source 📝

319:
letters, colors, and shapes. For example, if he told the participants beforehand to report on the numbers observed, then they were unable to describe the letters, colors, or shapes with any accuracy after the experiment. If he told participants to describe the colors, then with subsequent questioning they were unable to describe the letters, numbers, or shapes. The item people could describe with the highest level of accuracy was always the item they were instructed to observe. These results indicated that the subjects of the experiment would abstract the requested features, while at the same time remaining “unconscious” to all of the other present features. Külpe wrote in a letter about the conclusions from these experiments: "The old doctrine of an inner sense with the involved idea of a distinction between the reality of consciousness and objectivity must now have its opportune renewal in the domain of psychology. This is the principal conclusion I would draw from my experiments. In connection there with I define abstraction as the process by which the logically or psychologically effective is separated from the logically or psychologically ineffective. The effective partial contents are positively abstracted for thinking and ideation. The ineffective are those from which the abstraction is made. For our consciousness, therefore, there are abstract ideas, for psychical reality there are only concrete ideas. At the same time the old controversy between nominalism and realism approaches its solution."
412:. In this book, he was looking back upon his focus on the systematic experimental introspection method. He believed that before systematic experimental introspection existed, research on thought was incomplete. He also believed that having the subject report only on sensations, feelings, and presentations in thought research was severely limiting in that it did not provide an acceptable opportunity to identify what could be considered neither sensation, feeling, nor presentation. Once they were given the opportunity to objectively self-observe and describe what was neither sensation, feeling, nor presentation, yet was still a thought process; Külpe and his colleagues identified the need for new definitions and concepts aside from those that already exist. As Külpe wrote: “The subjects began to speak in the language of life and assign less importance to the presentations for their inner world. They knew and thought, they judged and understood, they grasped the meaning and saw connections without receiving any real help from occasionally occurring imagery.” 323:
this case, the contents are the thoughts themselves, whereas the acts or functions are the thinking process. To Külpe, function and content are different and independently variable. He proposed that the acts or functions are not analyzable in consciousness, are relatively unstable, and can only be observed and known after an event has occurred. In order to observe them, Külpe's process of systematic experimental introspection is critical. Külpe believed that contents, on the other hand, are analyzable in consciousness, observable in introspection, and relatively stable. He also cautioned against speculating about what is occurring physiologically at the time of thinking. Instead, Külpe encouraged the finding and improving of facts on the subject. That is, ignore the physiological processes occurring as part of the thinking process but instead introspect upon what you know has occurred. As a result of his experiment, Külpe determined that visual perception is determined not only by external stimulation but also by
310:
neither be described nor associated in the mind with an image. Once they were given the opportunity to objectively self-observe and describe what was neither sensation, feeling, nor presentation, yet was still a thought process Külpe and his colleagues identified the need for new definitions and concepts aside from those that already exist. For example, Külpe and his students, A.M. Mayer and J. Orth identified that following the presentation of the stimulus word “meter”, an indescribable conscious process occurred that led to the subject responding with the word “trochee”. This, they proposed, indicated that Wundt was wrong in his belief that all events in the thought process have either associated or direct images. Their research, although imperfect, using the systematic experimental introspection methods that Külpe and the students had developed and refined, established a foundation for imageless thought research that is still relevant and debated within the field of psychology today.
228:. It was his innovative experimental psychology methods and success in establishing these psychological institutes that led to Külpe being referred to as the second founder of experimental psychology on German soil. Though Külpe and Wundt differed on matters of principle, Külpe regarded Wundt highly and published three tributes to him. In his later years, Külpe began to focus less on psychological issues and more on his interests in philosophical problems such as esthetics, where it seemed his true passion lay. Just before the Christmas of 1915, Külpe suffered a bout of influenza. He recovered to the point where he was able to return to his university teachings. He was, however, suffering from a heart infection, which he succumbed to on 30 December, after a few days of illness. It was said that had he lived longer, Külpe likely would have accepted the request to succeed Friederich Jodl and establish a psychological laboratory at the 397:). This book was a guide to both past and present philosophy at the time, and a basic text for German university students of not just general philosophy, but psychology, logic, ethics, and aesthetics as well. In the book, Külpe also looks at the relations of the body and the mind, and in doing so, takes a dualistic position. Külpe also gives a clearly describes the relationship between physical and psychical, or in other words natural science and psychology. He identifies the possibility of what he refers to as a mind substance. He theorized that both matter and mind are abstractions from thought experience. He believed that if matter required the idea of substance, why would mind not require substance as well. The book is less than 350 pages and went through seven editions and four translations, including into English by W. B. Pillsbury and Titchener under the title 220:, before being promoted to a professorship extraordinarius in 1894. In October 1894, he attended Würzburg as professor ordinarius, the highest obtainable rank as a professor at a German university, for both philosophy and esthetics. It was here in 1896 that Külpe founded a psychological laboratory. He was able to increase the size and improve the equipment until the Würzburg laboratory became the most outstanding institute of psychology in Germany aside from Leipzig. Here, he trained numerous influential psychologists including the likes of Max Wertheimer, Kaspar Ach, and Henry Watt. Following fifteen years of service at the lead of the Würzburg laboratory, Külpe also established first-rate psychological institutes at the 293:
psychology made by the Würzburg school. The emphasis on motivation, and the role that it plays in the results of thinking was stressed at the school and is still relevant today. Today, it is still a widely accepted principle that motivation is a variable that affects thinking outcomes. Another contribution from the school was the theory that the behaviour of the ‘id’ depended not only on the element within the thinker's consciousness, but that there were unconscious determinants of behaviour as well. This is another notable idea born from the Würzburg school that is still relevant and widely accepted in psychology today.
196:, one of the Baltic providences of the Russian Empire. However, his father, a notary, and his mother were German. Consequently, Külpe's native tongue was German. He had a brother, Alfons Külpe who was a clergyman, and a sister who was a nurse. He lived a large portion of his life with his older, unmarried cousins, Ottillie and Marie Külpe, at their residences in Leipzig, Würzburg, Bonn, and Munich. He never married either and throughout the years he devoted an immense amount of time to his work. It was said that Külpe would joke that science was his bride. He learned Russian during his training at the 306:. He used an experiment to demonstrate that our ability to recognize something one has seen before is unrelated to whether or not we can remember an image of it. In his demonstrative experiment he took participants into a darkened room and asked them to visualize colors as he called them out. In all situations but one participants were able to visualize the colors. The participant that was unable to visualize the colors had no cognitive deficits, which lead Külpe to his conclusion that recognition is independent of remembrance 382:. The handbook summarized a comprehensive amount of experimental research at the time, including new research on reaction time, contributions to psychophysics, Carl Stumpf's research into tonal fusions, and Hermann Ebbinghaus’ research on memory. He defined psychology as, “the facts of experience," as the book concerned itself strictly with scientific fact. The lack of focus on the concept of thought in his book is interesting because the Würzburg school greatly researched mental set and imageless thought. He once wrote to a 386:, however, that the concepts behind the book were, “the source of the investigations in the psychology of thinking.” So, he had anticipated the idea of imageless thought in this publication. Külpe was still under the shadow of Wundt during this time. In fact, only one of his distinctive views was included, which was his criticism of the subtractive procedure on reaction time. Two years later, Grundriss der Psychologie was translated into English by Edward Titchener under the title Outlines of Psychology. 252:, a key area of focus was the development and formation of concepts. Külpe and his students expanded the use of introspection and were the first to research thought processes using experimental methods (Watson, 1978). In doing so, they developed and improved the process of what became known as systematic experimental introspection, which was the retrospective reporting of the experiences of a subject after performing a complex task involving thinking, remembering, or judging. 344:
Külpe and his Würzburg associates also used his abstraction experiments to reject associationism as the elementary thinking method. For example, in situations where participants were asked to provide a superordinate category, or superior group within a classification system, for birds, they were more
335:
between the range of attention and degree of consciousness. He found that the wider one's span of attention, the lower one's degree of consciousness is to any specific aspect, and vice versa. He concluded that there is a limited amount of energy driving attention and that this limitation is constant.
318:
In the early 1900s Külpe performed experiments on the concept of abstraction at the Würzburg School. Külpe defined abstraction as a process in which one focuses on certain aspects of reality while ignoring others. In one famous experiment Külpe instructed participants to observe a display of numbers,
322:
In another letter, Külpe identified a key distinction in his ideas as compared to Wundt's ideas. Külpe differentiates 'thought' from 'thinking'. Thoughts are 'contents’, he argues, whereas thinking in its various forms such as meaning, judging, and concluding can be considered acts or functions. In
301:
Perhaps the most famous psychological contribution to come out of the Würzburg Laboratory was related to Külpe's beliefs in philosophical realism. The work focused on the idea of imageless thought, which is the belief that there is an objective significance that can be found within experiences that
292:
Distinct from the view of Wundt, the Würzburg school developed an innovational, holistic view, where the focus was on studying both act and content. This research established a strong foundation for the Gestalt psychologists who were to come. This research was not the only notable contributions to
256:
experiments were especially important in distinguishing relevant features of objects for individuals in differing stages of development. Throughout his time at Würzburg he acted as a devoted teacher and administrator. In fact, most of his prestige comes from his dedication to his students and the
215:
Following his time with Müller, he returned to Leipzig to study under Wundt as his assistant in the autumn of 1886. On 12 October 1887 Külpe was awarded his PhD. He titled his thesis “Zur Theorie der sinnlichen Gefühle”, or in English, The Theory of Sensual Feeling. This is a topic that Külpe was
309:
Külpe believed that the research on thought processes up to that point, including Wundt's study on the associations between thoughts and images, had been incomplete. Influenced by his interest in philosophy, Külpe believed that there were certain sensations, feelings, or presentations that could
260:
Though he published many of his own personal works, Külpe never penned any of the traditional Würzburg papers, so it is difficult to specify his personal interpretation of the work of the school. However, in many of his personal works he often anticipated notions that were later discussed by his
184:, though his ideas differed from Wundt as he developed his own research (Boring, 1961). Külpe made significant contributions to the field of psychology, some of which are still relevant, including the systematic experimental introspection, imageless thoughts, mental sets, and abstraction. 177:, revolutionized experimental psychology at his time. In his obituary, Aloys Fischer wrote that, “undoubtedly Külpe was the second founder of experimental psychology on German soil; for with every change of base he made it a requirement that an experimental laboratory should be provided.” 200:, where he graduated in 1879. He then taught history and other subjects at a boys’ school for a year and a half before relocating to Leipzig. In 1881 he enrolled in the University of Leipzig. He focused his studies mostly on history, however he attended the lectures of 349:
laboratory concluded that behavior such as the above example could not be explained according to associationistic logic. They determined that the actual task, instead of the stimulus directs the thinking process. This mechanism became known as the
354:. Specifically the mental set refers to an innate tendency to respond a certain way. Eventually the mental set was seen as a factor that could account for a large portion of the variation in the ways that people solve problems. 208:, where he attended the lectures of Heinrich von Treitschke, a nationalist German historian. Following his studies in Berlin, he transferred to Göttingen, where he spent two years as a student of 762: 204:. In these he became familiar with the blossoming field of psychology, the area upon which his life work eventually would be focused. In between 1882 and 1883, Külpe studied at the 772: 336:
He also concluded that the abstraction process is based on apprehension, rather than sensational differences in presentation, and that these two concepts are distinct.
752: 722: 426:, which was presented in the year of his untimely death, 1915, before the Bavarian Academy of Science. Other books by Külpe include the 1912 publication of 302:
are not necessarily associated with specific words, symbols or signs. Külpe anticipated the notion of imageless thought in his early work as evidenced in
757: 216:
interested in over the course of his life, and especially influenced his later studies and lectures on esthetics. He then became Privatdozent at the
533:
Eling, P., Derckx, K., & Maes, R. "On the historical and conceptual background of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test". Brain and Cognition. 2008
418:
a three-volume text composed of Külpe's lectures, was published from 1912 through 1923. Other notable publications include the publication of a
651:
Külpe, O. (1897). Introduction to philosophy: Psychology, logic, ethics, aesthetics and general philosophy. London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co.
615: 742: 737: 717: 558:
Leahey, T. (1987). A history of psychology: Main currents in psychological thought (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
345:
likely to respond by saying, ‘Animal’ than a specific bird such as a ‘hummingbird.’ As a result, Külpe and the students at the
609: 521:
King, D.B., Viney, W., and Woody, W. D. "A History of Psychology: Ideas and Context." Pearson Education. 2009. pg. 262-263.
732: 248:
and he managed to make it one of the foremost psychological institutes second only to the University of Leipzig. At the
747: 585:
Fraisse, P, Piaget, J, & Reuchlin, M. (1963). Experimental psychology: its scope and method. New York: Basic Books.
777: 663:
Külpe, O. (1912). Vorlesungen über Psychologic. Über die moderne Psychologic des Denkens. (2nd ed.). Leipzig: Hirzel.
505:
Lindenfeld, D. "Oswald Külpe and The Würzburg School". The Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. 1978
727: 630: 767: 712: 362:
Oswald Külpe's books and published works cover a variety of subject matter, which impacted his interest in
449:
Ogden, R. M. (1951). Oswald Külpe and the Würzburg School. The American Journal of Psychology, 64, 4–19.
684: 567:
Pratt, C. C. "The Stability of Aesthetic Judgments". The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism. 1956
576:
Murray, D. (1988). A history of Western psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
241: 608: 165: 678: 39: 217: 181: 170: 485:
Ogden, R.M." Oswald Külpe and the Würzburg School". The American Journal of Psychology. 1951
261:
students. Külpe managed to exert his influence on the field of psychology via his students.
209: 173:
of the late 19th and early 20th century. Külpe, who is lesser known than his German mentor,
707: 702: 229: 225: 205: 8: 549:
Watson, R. (1978). The great psychologists (4th ed.). New York: J. B. Lippincott Company.
278: 245: 282: 266: 221: 197: 82: 249: 92: 269:
was undoubtedly his most famous student. However other noteworthy students include
116: 262: 131: 145: 696: 604: 594:
Külpe, O. (1893). Outlines of psychology. London: Swan Sonnenschein & Co.
274: 174: 121: 72: 270: 375: 201: 332: 331:(task) slightly in each session of the experiment, he was able to find a 286: 253: 135: 619:. Vol. 18 (11th ed.). Cambridge University Press. p. 242. 346: 371: 363: 351: 327:, which is another word for the task or directive. Since he varied the 212:, where it is said he received the idea for his doctoral dissertation. 109: 419: 677: 193: 180:
Külpe studied as a doctoral student and assistant to Wundt at the
383: 35: 61: 367: 763:
Academic staff of the Ludwig Maximilian University of Munich
244:
and remained there for fifteen years. He received a private
773:
Emigrants from the Russian Empire to the German Empire
277:, both of whom worked on the concept of mental set; 257:hard work he directed towards his teaching duties. 659: 657: 545: 543: 541: 539: 192:In August 1862, Oswald Külpe was born in Kandau, 169:; 3 August 1862 – 30 December 1915) was a German 694: 378:. His first major book, published in 1893, was 654: 536: 240:By 1896 Külpe had founded a laboratory at the 366:. Examples of his publication topics include 289:, one of the founders of the Gestalt school. 753:Academic staff of the University of Würzburg 723:Baltic-German people from the Russian Empire 501: 499: 497: 495: 493: 491: 313: 389:In 1895, Külpe published a handbook called 16:German structural psychologist (1862–1915) 488: 281:, who played a major role in introducing 235: 758:Academic staff of the University of Bonn 603: 481: 479: 477: 475: 406:Über die moderne Psychologie des Denkens 529: 527: 473: 471: 469: 467: 465: 463: 461: 459: 457: 455: 695: 517: 515: 513: 511: 164: 524: 452: 410:On the Modern Psychology of Thinking 296: 508: 408:, which translates into English as 13: 14: 789: 670: 743:20th-century German philosophers 738:19th-century German philosophers 718:People from Courland Governorate 645: 623: 597: 588: 579: 570: 561: 552: 443: 357: 1: 436: 391:Einleitung in die Philosophie 339: 631:"Introduction to Philosophy" 187: 158:Theodor Oswald Rudolph Külpe 7: 142:Other notable students 10: 794: 733:Experimental psychologists 432:Philosophie der Gegenwart. 399:Introduction to Philosophy 395:Introduction to Philosophy 285:to the United States; and 748:Leipzig University alumni 404:In 1912, Külpe published 380:Grundriss der Psychologie 314:Abstraction and attention 304:Grundriss der Psychologie 151: 141: 127: 115: 105: 98: 88: 78: 68: 46: 28: 21: 778:Deaths from endocarditis 685:Encyclopædia Britannica 616:Encyclopædia Britannica 428:Psychologie und Medizin 171:structural psychologist 688:(12th ed.). 1922. 242:University of Würzburg 236:Laboratory at Würzburg 679:"Külpe, Oswald"  393:(which translates to 218:University of Leipzig 182:University of Leipzig 728:German psychologists 230:University of Vienna 226:University of Munich 206:University of Berlin 768:German male writers 713:People from Kandava 610:"Metaphysics"  424:Zur Katagorienlehre 279:Robert Morris Ogden 374:, philosophy, and 283:Gestalt psychology 267:Gestalt psychology 222:University of Bonn 210:Georg Elias Müller 198:Gymnasium in Libau 83:Leipzig University 416:Die Realisierung, 297:Imageless thought 265:, the founder of 155: 154: 128:Doctoral students 100:Scientific career 785: 689: 681: 664: 661: 652: 649: 643: 642: 640: 638: 627: 621: 620: 612: 601: 595: 592: 586: 583: 577: 574: 568: 565: 559: 556: 550: 547: 534: 531: 522: 519: 506: 503: 486: 483: 450: 447: 168: 166:[ˈkylpə] 163: 117:Doctoral advisor 57: 55: 50:30 December 1915 19: 18: 793: 792: 788: 787: 786: 784: 783: 782: 693: 692: 676: 673: 668: 667: 662: 655: 650: 646: 636: 634: 629: 628: 624: 602: 598: 593: 589: 584: 580: 575: 571: 566: 562: 557: 553: 548: 537: 532: 525: 520: 509: 504: 489: 484: 453: 448: 444: 439: 360: 342: 316: 299: 250:Würzburg school 238: 190: 161: 93:Würzburg School 79:Alma mater 64: 59: 53: 51: 42: 33: 24: 17: 12: 11: 5: 791: 781: 780: 775: 770: 765: 760: 755: 750: 745: 740: 735: 730: 725: 720: 715: 710: 705: 691: 690: 672: 671:External links 669: 666: 665: 653: 644: 633:. adlibris.com 622: 607:, ed. (1911). 605:Chisholm, Hugh 596: 587: 578: 569: 560: 551: 535: 523: 507: 487: 451: 441: 440: 438: 435: 359: 356: 341: 338: 315: 312: 298: 295: 263:Max Wertheimer 237: 234: 189: 186: 153: 152: 149: 148: 143: 139: 138: 132:Max Wertheimer 129: 125: 124: 119: 113: 112: 107: 103: 102: 96: 95: 90: 89:Known for 86: 85: 80: 76: 75: 70: 66: 65: 60: 58:(aged 53) 48: 44: 43: 34: 30: 26: 25: 22: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 790: 779: 776: 774: 771: 769: 766: 764: 761: 759: 756: 754: 751: 749: 746: 744: 741: 739: 736: 734: 731: 729: 726: 724: 721: 719: 716: 714: 711: 709: 706: 704: 701: 700: 698: 687: 686: 680: 675: 674: 660: 658: 648: 632: 626: 618: 617: 611: 606: 600: 591: 582: 573: 564: 555: 546: 544: 542: 540: 530: 528: 518: 516: 514: 512: 502: 500: 498: 496: 494: 492: 482: 480: 478: 476: 474: 472: 470: 468: 466: 464: 462: 460: 458: 456: 446: 442: 434: 433: 429: 425: 421: 417: 413: 411: 407: 402: 400: 396: 392: 387: 385: 381: 377: 373: 369: 365: 355: 353: 348: 337: 334: 330: 326: 320: 311: 307: 305: 294: 290: 288: 284: 280: 276: 272: 268: 264: 258: 255: 251: 247: 243: 233: 231: 227: 223: 219: 213: 211: 207: 203: 199: 195: 185: 183: 178: 176: 175:Wilhelm Wundt 172: 167: 159: 150: 147: 144: 140: 137: 133: 130: 126: 123: 122:Wilhelm Wundt 120: 118: 114: 111: 108: 104: 101: 97: 94: 91: 87: 84: 81: 77: 74: 73:Baltic German 71: 67: 63: 49: 45: 41: 37: 32:3 August 1862 31: 27: 20: 683: 647: 635:. Retrieved 625: 614: 599: 590: 581: 572: 563: 554: 445: 431: 427: 423: 415: 414: 409: 405: 403: 398: 394: 390: 388: 379: 376:epistemology 361: 343: 328: 324: 321: 317: 308: 303: 300: 291: 259: 239: 214: 202:Wilhem Wundt 191: 179: 157: 156: 99: 23:Oswald Külpe 708:1915 deaths 703:1862 births 358:Major works 333:correlation 287:Kurt Koffka 254:Abstraction 146:Karl Bühler 136:Ernst Bloch 69:Nationality 697:Categories 437:References 372:aesthetics 364:psychology 352:mental set 340:Mental set 275:Henry Watt 271:Narziß Ach 110:Psychology 54:1915-12-31 420:monograph 384:colleague 246:endowment 232:as well. 188:Biography 347:Würzburg 224:and the 194:Courland 40:Courland 637:15 June 329:Aufgabe 325:Aufgabe 162:German: 52: ( 106:Fields 62:Munich 36:Kandau 368:logic 639:2023 430:and 273:and 47:Died 29:Born 699:: 682:. 656:^ 613:. 538:^ 526:^ 510:^ 490:^ 454:^ 422:, 401:. 370:, 134:, 38:, 641:. 160:( 56:)

Index

Kandau
Courland
Munich
Baltic German
Leipzig University
Würzburg School
Psychology
Doctoral advisor
Wilhelm Wundt
Max Wertheimer
Ernst Bloch
Karl Bühler
[ˈkylpə]
structural psychologist
Wilhelm Wundt
University of Leipzig
Courland
Gymnasium in Libau
Wilhem Wundt
University of Berlin
Georg Elias Müller
University of Leipzig
University of Bonn
University of Munich
University of Vienna
University of Würzburg
endowment
Würzburg school
Abstraction
Max Wertheimer

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.