Knowledge

Errorless learning

Source šŸ“

27: 134:
a green key (Sāˆ’). Using traditional procedures, a pigeon would be initially trained to peck a red key (S+). When the pigeon was responding consistently to the red key (S+), a green key (Sāˆ’) would be introduced. At first the pigeon would also respond to the green key (Sāˆ’) but gradually responses to this key would decrease, because they are not followed by food, so that they occurred only a few times or even never.
98:...errors are not necessary for learning to occur. Errors are not a function of learning or vice versa nor are they blamed on the learner. Errors are a function of poor analysis of behavior, a poorly designed shaping program, moving too fast from step to step in the program, and the lack of the prerequisite behavior necessary for success in the program. 142:
words, the S+ and Sāˆ’ were initially presented with different brightness and duration, i.e., the S+ would appear for 5 s and fully red, and the Sāˆ’ would appear for 0.5 s and dark. Gradually, over successive presentations, the duration of the Sāˆ’ and its brightness were gradually increased until the keylight was fully green for 5 s.
162:
The errorless learning procedure is highly effective in reducing the number of responses to the Sāˆ’ during training. In Terrace's (1963) experiment, subjects trained with the conventional discrimination procedure averaged over 3000 Sāˆ’ (errors) responses during 28 sessions of training; whereas subjects
133:
A simple discrimination learning procedure is one in which a subject learns to associate one stimulus, S+ (positive stimulus), with reinforcement (e.g. food) and another, Sāˆ’ (negative stimulus), with extinction (e.g. absence of food). For example, a pigeon can learn to peck a red key (S+), and avoid
153:
from cognitive psychology and cognitive neuropsychology have provided additional theoretical support for errorless learning methods (e.g., Brooks and Baddeley, 1976, Tulving and Schacter, 1990). Implicit memory is known to be poor at eliminating errors, but can be used to compensate when explicit
141:
and visual stimuli (S+ and Sāˆ’) like colors are used that differ in terms of brightness, duration and wavelength. He used a fading procedure in which the brightness and duration differences between the S+ and the Sāˆ’ were decreased progressively leaving only the difference in wavelength. In other
203:
Errorless learning has been found to be effective in helping memory-impaired people learn more effectively. The reason for the method's effectiveness is that, while those with sufficient memory function can remember mistakes and learn from them, those with memory impairment may have difficulty
183:
However, some evidence suggests that errorless learning may not be as qualitatively different from conventional training as Terrace initially claimed. For example, Rilling (1977) demonstrated in a series of experiments that these "by-products" can occur after errorless learning, but that their
166:
Later, Terrace (1972) claimed not only that the errorless learning procedure improves long-term discrimination performance, but also that: 1) Sāˆ’ does not become aversive and so does not elicit "aggressive" behaviors, as it often does with conventional training; 2) Sāˆ’ does not develop
204:
remembering not only which methods work, but may strengthen incorrect responses over correct responses, such as via emotional stimuli. See also the reference by Brown to its application in teaching mathematics to undergraduates.
154:
memory function is impaired. In experiments on amnesiac patients, errorless implicit learning was more effective because it reduced the possibility of errors "sticking" in amnesiacs' memories.
184:
effects may not be as large as in the conventional procedure; and Marsh and Johnson (1968) found that subjects given errorless training were very slow to make a discrimination reversal.
192:
Interest from psychologists studying basic research on errorless learning declined after the 1970s. However, errorless learning attracted the interest of researchers in
175:
to S+ does not occur. In other words, Terrace has claimed that the "by-products" of conventional discrimination learning do not occur with the errorless procedure.
125:). In discrimination learning, an error is a response to the Sāˆ’, and according to Terrace errors are not required for successful discrimination performance. 232:
Baddeley, A.D. and Wilson, B.A. (1994) When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32(1), 53-68.
243: 70: 48: 41: 200:
patients). Errorless learning continues to be of practical interest to animal trainers, particularly dog trainers.
137:
Terrace (1963) found that discrimination learning could occur without errors when the training begins early in
298:
Rilling, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In: W.K. Honing & J.E.R Staddon (Orgs.),
163:
trained with the errorless procedure averaged only 25 Sāˆ’ (errors) responses in the same number of sessions.
263:
B. Wilson (2009) Memory Rehabilitation: Integrating Theory and Practice, The Guilford Press, 284 pages.
279: 196:, and studies have been conducted with both children (e.g., educational settings) and adults (e.g. 35: 117:
to occur with few or even with no responses to the negative stimulus (abbreviated Sāˆ’). A negative
213: 114: 90:
in the 1950s as part of his studies on what would make the most effective learning environment.
52: 118: 138: 8: 172: 305:
Skinner, B. F. (1937). Two types of conditioned reflex: a reply to Konorski and Miller.
193: 102:
Errorless learning can also be understood at a synaptic level, using the principle of
342: 150: 312:
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
109:
Many of Skinner's other students and followers continued to test the idea. In 1963,
325:
Terrace, H.S. (1972). By-products of discrimination learning. In G.H. Bower (Ed.),
103: 146: 110: 87: 197: 336: 122: 91: 272: 318:
Terrace, H.S. (1963). Discrimination learning with and without "errors".
121:
is a stimulus associated with undesirable consequences (e.g., absence of
315:
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York: Macmillan.
168: 275:(2012) Using these ideas for undergraduate teaching of mathematics! 113:
wrote a paper describing an experiment with pigeons which allows
94:
was also influential in developing the technique, noting that,
284:
Rosales Ruiz, J. (2007). 'Teaching Dogs the Clicker Way' In:
302:(pp. 432ā€“480). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 86:
was an instructional design introduced by psychologist
295:. 6th edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. 241: 334: 320:Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 106:("Neurons that fire together wire together"). 327:The psychology of learning and motivation 71:Learn how and when to remove this message 34:This article includes a list of general 335: 273:Getting students not to fear confusion 16:Instructional learning without errors 20: 329:(Vol. 5). New York: Academic Press. 13: 40:it lacks sufficient corresponding 14: 354: 242:Jesus Rosales-Ruiz (July 2011). 25: 244:"Teaching Dogs the Clicker Way" 187: 257: 235: 226: 1: 307:Journal of General Psychology 219: 128: 300:Handbook of operant behavior 7: 207: 10: 359: 157: 178: 171:properties; 3) positive 214:Evidence-based learning 115:discrimination learning 55:more precise citations. 286:Teaching Dogs Magazine 278:BF Skinner biography. 100: 293:Learning and behavior 96: 291:Mazur, J.E. (2006). 139:operant conditioning 173:behavioral contrast 194:applied psychology 84:Errorless learning 151:implicit learning 81: 80: 73: 350: 288:, May/June 2007. 264: 261: 255: 254: 248: 239: 233: 230: 104:Hebbian learning 76: 69: 65: 62: 56: 51:this article by 42:inline citations 29: 28: 21: 358: 357: 353: 352: 351: 349: 348: 347: 333: 332: 268: 267: 262: 258: 246: 240: 236: 231: 227: 222: 210: 190: 181: 160: 147:implicit memory 131: 111:Herbert Terrace 88:Charles Ferster 77: 66: 60: 57: 47:Please help to 46: 30: 26: 17: 12: 11: 5: 356: 346: 345: 331: 330: 323: 316: 313: 310: 309:, 16, 272-279. 303: 296: 289: 282: 276: 266: 265: 256: 234: 224: 223: 221: 218: 217: 216: 209: 206: 189: 186: 180: 177: 159: 156: 130: 127: 79: 78: 33: 31: 24: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 355: 344: 341: 340: 338: 328: 324: 321: 317: 314: 311: 308: 304: 301: 297: 294: 290: 287: 283: 281: 277: 274: 270: 269: 260: 252: 251:Teaching Dogs 245: 238: 229: 225: 215: 212: 211: 205: 201: 199: 195: 185: 176: 174: 170: 164: 155: 152: 148: 143: 140: 135: 126: 124: 123:reinforcement 120: 116: 112: 107: 105: 99: 95: 93: 92:B. F. Skinner 89: 85: 75: 72: 64: 54: 50: 44: 43: 37: 32: 23: 22: 19: 326: 319: 306: 299: 292: 285: 280:B.F. Skinner 259: 250: 237: 228: 202: 191: 188:Applications 182: 165: 161: 144: 136: 132: 108: 101: 97: 83: 82: 67: 58: 39: 18: 198:Parkinson's 145:Studies of 61:August 2010 53:introducing 322:, 6, 1ā€“27. 271:R. Brown, 220:References 169:inhibitory 129:Principles 36:references 343:Learning 337:Category 208:See also 119:stimulus 158:Effects 49:improve 179:Limits 38:, but 247:(PDF) 149:and 339:: 249:. 253:. 74:) 68:( 63:) 59:( 45:.

Index

references
inline citations
improve
introducing
Learn how and when to remove this message
Charles Ferster
B. F. Skinner
Hebbian learning
Herbert Terrace
discrimination learning
stimulus
reinforcement
operant conditioning
implicit memory
implicit learning
inhibitory
behavioral contrast
applied psychology
Parkinson's
Evidence-based learning
"Teaching Dogs the Clicker Way"
Getting students not to fear confusion
B.F. Skinner
Category
Learning

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

ā†‘