Knowledge

Antisymmetry

Source 📝

924: 1227:, or even the Phonetic Form, i.e. the order in which sentences are pronounced in natural speech. This rejects the idea of an underlying ordering that is then subject to movement. In a 2008 article, Marc Richards argued that a head parameter must only reside at PF, as it is unmaintainable in its original form as a structural parameter. In this approach the relative positions of head and complement that are found at this surface level, which show variation both between and within languages (see above), must be treated as the "true" orderings. 171:. Kayne uses a simple definition of c-command based on the "first node up". However, the definition is complicated by his use of a "segment/category" distinction. Two directly connected nodes that have the same label are "segments" of a single "category". A category "excludes" all categories not "dominated" by all its segments. A "c-commands" B if every category that dominates A also dominates B, and A excludes B. The following tree illustrates these concepts: 40: 740:, "with an ordered triple replacing the two ordered pairs and then being mappable to a ternary-branching tree" (pp. 17). Kayne goes on to say, "This would lead to seeing my arguments for binary branching to have two subcomponents, the first being the claim that syntax is n-ary branching with n having a single value, the second being that that value is 2. Mapping [ 264:(with the exception that (i.e., the specifier of the CP phrase) is treated as an adjunct). It can be seen that removing any of the structures in the tree (e.g., deleting the C dominating the 'c' terminal, so that the complement of A is ) will destroy the asymmetric c-command relations necessary for linearly ordering the terminals of the tree. 387:), which appears at the end of the sentence in questions. It is generally assumed that languages such as English have a "covert" (i.e. phonologically null) equivalent of this particle in the 'C' position of the clause — the position just to the right of . This particle is overtly realised in English by the movement of an 1367:
Thus Takita shows that surface head-final structures in Japanese do not block movement, as they do in Chinese. He concludes that, because it does not block movement as shown in previous sections, Japanese is a genuinely head-final language, and not derived from an underlying, head-initial structure.
1235:
Takita argues against the conclusion of Kayne's Antisymmetry Theory, which states that all languages are head-initial at an underlying level. He claims that a language such as Japanese is truly head-final since the mass movement required to take an underlying head-initial structure to the head-final
189:
are both segments of a single category. AP does not c-command BP because it does not exclude BP. CP does not c-command BP because both segments of AP do not dominate BP (so it is not the case that every category that dominates CP dominates BP). BP c-commands CP and A. A c-commands C. The definitions
409:
Now it is clear why Japanese does not have wh-movement — the position is already filled, so no wh-phrase can move to it. The relationship between surface word order and the possibility of wh-movement is seemingly obscure. A possible alternative to the antisymmetric explanation could be based
298:
Perhaps the biggest challenge for antisymmetry is to explain the wide variety of different surface orders across languages. Any deviation from Spec-Head-Comp order (which implies overall Subject-Verb-Object order, if objects are complements) must be explained by movement. Kayne argues that in some
272:
Kayne notes that his theory permits either a universal specifier-head-complement order or a universal complement-head-specifier order, depending on whether asymmetric c-command establishes precedence or subsequence (S-H-C results from precedence). He prefers S-H-C as the universal underlying order
1029:
meaning, and thus to be the head of an aspect phrase having the verb phrase as its complement. If phrases are always essentially head-initial, then a case like this must entail movement, since the particle comes after the verb phrase. It is proposed that there the complement moves into specifier
207:
Informally, Kayne's theory states that if a nonterminal category A asymmetrically c-commands another nonterminal category B, all the terminal nodes dominated by A must precede all of the terminal nodes dominated by B (this statement is commonly referred to as the "Linear Correspondence Axiom" or
943:
and production. Parsing and production proceed in a left-to-right direction: the beginning of the sentence is heard or spoken first, and the end of the sentence is heard or spoken last. This implies (according to the theory) an ordering whereby probe comes before the goal, i.e. head precedes
848:
Kayne argues that a theory that allows both directionalities would state that languages are symmetrical, whereas in fact languages are found to be asymmetrical in many respects. Examples of linguistic asymmetries which may be cited in support of the theory (although they do not concern head
1475: 429:(PF). The unwanted structures are then rescued by movement: deleting the phonetic content of the moved element neutralizes the linearization problem. Dynamic Antisymmetry aims at unifying a movement and phrase structure, which otherwise are independent properties. 1363:
In (b), the fronted VP precedes the matrix subject, confirming that the VP is located in the matrix clause. If Japanese were head-initial, (b) should not be grammatical because it allows for the extraction of an element (VP2) from the moved complement (CP2).
1236:
ones actually found in such languages would violate other constraints. It is implied that such languages are likely following a head-final parameter value, as originally conceived. (For a head-initial/Antisymmetry analysis of Japanese, see Kayne.)
307:
is the most striking example of this. From the mid-1980s onwards, the standard analysis of wh-movement involved the wh-phrase moving leftward to a position on the left edge of the clause called . Thus, a derivation of the English question
190:
above may perhaps be thought to allow BP to c-command AP, but a c-command relation is not usually assumed to hold between two such categories, and for the purposes of antisymmetry, the question of whether BP c-commands AP is in fact moot.
1239:
Takita's argument is based on Lin's analysis of Chinese. Since surface head-final structures are derived from underlying head-initial structures by moving the complements, further extraction from within the moved complement violates CED.
1656: 299:
cases the need for extra movements (previously unnecessary because different underlying orders were assumed for different languages) can explain some otherwise mysterious typological generalizations. His explanation for the lack of
840:
as such: it claims that at an underlying level, all languages are head-initial. In fact, it argued that all languages have the underlying order Specifier-Head-Complement. Deviations from this order are accounted for by different
1010:
Here, at each phrasal level in turn, the head of the phrase moves from left to right position relative to its complement. The eventual result reflects the ordering of complex nested phrases found in languages such as Japanese.
502:
involves immediate temporal precedence (or immediate linear precedence) so that H immediately precedes (i-precedes) C. Kayne proposes furthermore that when a specifier S merges, it forms an ordered pair with the head directly,
437:
Kayne proposed recasting the antisymmetry of natural language as a condition of "Merge", the operation which combines two elements into one. Kayne proposes that merging a head H and its complement C yields an ordered pair
870:, where the verb frequently fails to agree with the following plural noun, and in French and Italian compound tenses, where the past participle may agree with a preceding direct object but not with the following one. 987:, this would involve complex and massive leftward movement, which violates the ideal of grammatical simplicity. An example of the type of movement scheme that would need to be envisaged is provided by Tokizaki: 281:
and complements (though the order of heads and complements themselves is relatively free). He further argues that a movement approach to deriving non-S-H-C orders is appropriate since it derives asymmetries in
1598:
Elordieta, Arantzazu (2014). Biberauer, T.; Sheehan, M., eds. "On the relevance of the Head Parameter in a mixed OV language". Theoretical Approaches to Disharmonic Word Order (Oxford Scholarship Online), p.
175: 535:, or S i-precedes H. Invoking i-precedence prevents more than two elements from merging with H; only one element can i-precede H (the specifier), and H can i-precede only one element (the complement). 220: 788: 700: 586: 1562: 1243:
One of the examples of movement that Takita looks at is that of VP-fronting in Japanese. Grammatically, the sentence without VP-fronting, (a), and the sentence where the VP moves to the
1215:, is ungrammatical. Lin cites this and other related findings as evidence that the above analysis is correct, supporting the view that Chinese aspect phrases are deeply head-initial. 959:
The specifier, at first internal to the complement, is moved to the unoccupied position to the left of the head. In terms of merged pairs, this structure can also be represented as:
1372:
involves binary head-directionality, and is not antisymmetric. Takita briefly applies the same tests to Turkish, another seemingly head-final language, and reports similar results.
826: 738: 650: 618: 533: 500: 468: 1528:
Nolda, Andreas (2004). Topics Detached to the Left: On ‘Left Dislocation’, ‘Hanging Topic’, and Related Constructions in German. Berlin: ZAS Papers in Linguistics. pp. 423–448.
1437:
Since any rightward movement must also be downward movement if there are no rightward specifiers or right adjunction, and downward movement is generally assumed to be illicit.
425:, a weak version of antisymmetry, which allows the generation of non-LCA compatible structures (points of symmetry) before the hierarchical structure is linearized at 248:
As a result, there is no right adjunction, and hence in practice no rightward movement either. Furthermore, the underlying order must be specifier-head-complement.
216:
ordering of all terminal nodes — if it cannot consistently order all of the terminal nodes in a tree, the tree is illicit. Consider the following tree:
939:
acts as a probe and looks for a goal, namely its complement. Kayne proposes that the direction of the probe-goal search must share the direction of language
1064:, assuming the above analysis, since that verb phrase has moved into a non-complement (specifier) position, and thus further movement (such as that which 395:
to C). Why is it that this particle is on the left edge of the clause in English, but on the right edge in Japanese? Kayne suggests that in Japanese, the
1056:
at surface level); and (b) movement is not possible from within a non-complement (Huang's Condition on Extraction Domain or CED). This would imply that
399:(apart from the question particle in C) has moved to the position. So, the structure for the Japanese example above is something like the following: 193:(The above is not an exhaustive list of c-command relations in the tree, but covers all of those that are significant in the following exposition.) 58: 229:
In this tree, the set of pairs of nonterminal categories such that the first member of the pair asymmetrically c-commands the second member is:
983:
away from this underlying base order. It has been pointed out, though, that in predominantly head-final languages such as Japanese and
979:
Antisymmetry then leads to a universal Specifier-Head-Complement order. The varied ordering found in human languages are explained by
863: 856:
appear at the start of sentences, as in "Henry – I've known that guy for a long time". They are not found at the end of sentences.
1608:
Tokizaki, Hisao (2011). "The nature of linear information in the morphosyntax-PF interface". English Linguistics 28 (2), p. 238.
403: 1618: 1740: 743: 655: 541: 931:
In arguing for a universal underlying Head-Complement order, Kayne uses the concept of a probe-goal search (based on the
1565:". Acquisition of Relative Clauses : Processing, typology, and function. John Benjamins Publishing Company. p. 150. 1719: 76: 226:(S and S' may either be simplex structures like BP, or complex structures with specifiers and complements like CP.) 1806: 1416: 828:] would retain the first subcomponent and replace 2 by 3 in the second, arguably with no loss in restrictiveness". 1491:
Moro, A. 2000 Dynamic Antisymmetry, Linguistic Inquiry Monograph Series 38, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
28: 837: 793: 705: 927:
X-bar syntactic tree showing the movement of the specifier (S) relative to the head (H) and complement (C)
588:
is not mappable to a tree structure, since H would have two mothers, and that it has the consequence that
199:
is the relation that holds between two categories, A and B, if A c-commands B but B does not c-command A.
121:. Kayne hypothesizes that all phrases whose surface order is not specifier-head-complement have undergone 1801: 623: 591: 506: 473: 441: 1796: 1046: 912: 1223:
According to the "surface true" viewpoint, analysis of head direction must take place at the level of
908: 137: 948: 859: 133: 114: 145: 125:
that disrupt this underlying order. Others have posited specifier-complement-head as the basic
24: 1068:
is required to undergo at LF level) is not possible. Such a restriction on the occurrence of
20: 174: 1019: 904: 422: 149: 8: 283: 274: 164: 923: 219: 1816: 1775: 1735:. 28th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. University of Southern California. 980: 936: 932: 893: 842: 329:
The Japanese equivalent of this sentence is as follows (note the lack of wh-movement):
278: 141: 122: 54: 1779: 1767: 1736: 1715: 1369: 1042: 916: 304: 1685:
Kayne, Richard S. (2003). "Antisymmetry and Japanese". English Linguistics 20: 1–40.
1759: 1750:
Takita, Kensuke (February 2009). "If Chinese is head-initial, Japanese cannot be".
1015: 889: 880:
and Japanese) tend to differ from those that follow the noun: they more often lack
110: 102: 1515:
Kayne, Richard S. (1981) "Unambiguous Paths," in Robert May and Jan Koster (eds.)
1811: 1631: 1014:
An attempt to provide evidence for Kayne's scheme is made by Lin, who considered
877: 873: 287: 1045:, it is postulated that (a) adverbials of this type are subject to movement at 1026: 984: 881: 388: 168: 1763: 132:
Antisymmetry as a principle of word order is reliant on X-bar notions such as
1790: 1771: 1244: 853: 426: 1224: 973: 261: 118: 1050: 892:
and are more likely to be non-finite (this can be found, for example, in
418: 300: 90: 899:
Other areas in which asymmetries are found, according to Kayne, include
126: 1730: 1289:{} Taro-NOM {} Hanako-NOM {} book-ACC discard-even did C‍ think 160: 1712:
The Antisymmetry of Syntax. Linguistic Inquiry Monograph Twenty-Five
293: 1354:{} book-ACC discard-even Taro-NOM {} Hanako-NOM did C‍ think 290:
are not mirrored by any known "verb second-from-last" languages).
940: 410:
on the difficulty of parsing languages with rightward movement.
900: 98: 1476:
Effects of Short-Term Storage in Processing Rightward Movement
951:
of a phrase. He represents the relevant scheme as follows:
866:). Examples of this are found in English sentences such as 391:
to C (in the case of the example above, by the movement of
208:
LCA). Moreover, this principle must suffice to establish a
113:
follow a universal order, namely specifier-head-complement
1657:
Desymmetrization: Parametric variation at the PF-Interface
1230: 1049:(LF) level (even though, in Chinese, they do not display 140:, and the existence of order-altering mechanisms such as 1659:". The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 53 (2-3), p. 283. 1669: 1667: 1665: 783:{\displaystyle \langle S,H\rangle ,\langle H,C\rangle } 695:{\displaystyle \langle S,H\rangle ,\langle H,C\rangle } 581:{\displaystyle \langle S,H\rangle ,\langle H,C\rangle } 286:(such as the fact that "verb-second" languages such as 1632:
Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar
1060:
could not appear in a verb phrase with sentence-final
202: 1440: 796: 746: 708: 658: 626: 594: 544: 509: 476: 444: 432: 1688: 1662: 1637: 1619:
Complement-to-specifier movement in Mandarin Chinese
935:). The idea of probes and goals in syntax is that a 862:
is stronger when the noun phrase precedes the verb (
1580: 1568: 1499: 1497: 49:
may be too technical for most readers to understand
1543: 1531: 1463:(Second ed.). Arnold/Oxford University Press. 976:syntactic trees as shown in the adjacent diagram. 947:Kayne's theory also addresses the position of the 820: 782: 732: 694: 644: 612: 580: 527: 494: 462: 23:. For the property of mathematical relations, see 294:Derived orders: the case of Japanese wh-questions 1788: 1494: 1381: 652:would seem to be constituents. He suggests that 470:(rather than the standard symmetric set {H,C}). 1480:Storage and Computation in the Language Faculty 1414: 1458: 251: 117:. The theory builds on the foundation of the 109:. It asserts that grammatical hierarchies in 1563:Learning to produce Quechua relative clauses 815: 797: 777: 765: 759: 747: 727: 709: 689: 677: 671: 659: 639: 627: 607: 595: 575: 563: 557: 545: 522: 510: 489: 477: 457: 445: 1732:Why are there no directionality parameters? 1402:A Theory of the Morphology-Syntax Interface 383:Japanese has an overt "question particle" ( 241:}. This gives rise to the total ordering: 1474:Neeleman, Ad & Peter Ackema (2002). " 1399: 257: 77:Learn how and when to remove this message 61:, without removing the technical details. 1218: 922: 831: 273:since the most widely attested order in 1729:Kayne, Richard S. (February 12, 2010). 1318: 413: 349: 155: 1789: 1749: 1694: 1673: 1643: 1621:". Ms., National Tsing Hua University. 1465:(See p. 461 for the Japanese example.) 1231:Existence of true head-final languages 1203:("How has Zhangsan repaired the car?") 267: 260:of this article is in accordance with 1728: 1709: 1586: 1574: 1549: 1537: 1503: 1446: 1387: 1198: 1180: 821:{\displaystyle \langle S,H,C\rangle } 733:{\displaystyle \langle S,H,C\rangle } 367: 59:make it understandable to non-experts 1461:Introducing Transformational Grammar 1247:, (b), do not significantly differ. 1033:As evidence for this, Lin considers 33: 1482:. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Pages 219-256. 1123:"How does Zhangsan repair the car?" 1030:position, which precedes the head. 1025:. This particle is taken to convey 645:{\displaystyle \langle H,C\rangle } 613:{\displaystyle \langle S,H\rangle } 528:{\displaystyle \langle S,H\rangle } 495:{\displaystyle \langle H,C\rangle } 463:{\displaystyle \langle H,C\rangle } 203:Precedence and asymmetric c-command 13: 1519:. Dordrecht: Kluwer. Pages 143-183 1517:Levels of Syntactic Representation 1418:The Syntactic Domain of Anaphora. 433:Antisymmetry and ternary branching 312:would proceed roughly as follows: 218: 173: 19:For the property of matrices, see 14: 1828: 1752:Journal of East Asian Linguistics 1041:("how?"). Based on prior work by 1478:" In S. Nooteboom et al. (eds.) 972:This process can be mapped onto 836:Antisymmetry theory rejects the 38: 1679: 1649: 1624: 1611: 1602: 1592: 1555: 1522: 1509: 29:Antisymmetric (disambiguation) 16:Syntactic theory in linguistics 1485: 1468: 1452: 1431: 1408: 1393: 1211:co-occurs with sentence-final 1: 1375: 876:that precede the noun (as in 838:head-directionality parameter 338: 277:is for specifiers to precede 1617:Lin, Tzong-Hong J. (2006), " 1561:Courtney, Ellen H. (2011). " 373:John-wa nani-o kaimashita ka 7: 1655:Richards, Marc D. (2008). " 1197:{} Zhangsan how repair car 915:, and forward and backward 10: 1833: 1710:Kayne, Richard S. (1994). 1703: 868:There's books on the table 376:John-TOP what-ACC bought Q 252:Derivation of X-bar theory 107:The Antisymmetry of Syntax 18: 1764:10.1007/s10831-009-9038-z 1634:". PhD dissertation, MIT. 1630:Huang, C.-T. J. (1982). " 1368:These results imply that 1286:-sae shita to] omotteiru] 909:serial verb constructions 1415:Reinhart, Tanya (1979). 864:Greenberg's Universal 33 256:The example tree in the 148:theories (as opposed to 1807:Syntactic relationships 1207:Sentence (b), in which 1120:Zhangsan how repair car 1020:sentence-final particle 243:⟨b, a, c⟩ 1459:Jamal Ouhalla (1999). 1359:(Takita 2009 57: (33b) 1351:Taro-ga to] omotteiru 1339: 1331: 1324: 1313: 1302: 1294:(Takita 2009 57: (33a) 1278: 1270: 1262: 1254: 1173: 1165: 1157: 1147: 1139: 1131: 1105: 1097: 1087: 1079: 928: 845:applied by languages. 822: 784: 734: 696: 646: 614: 582: 529: 496: 464: 363: 355: 344: 333: 235:⟨BP, CP⟩ 223: 178: 163:is a relation between 146:constituency structure 27:. For other uses, see 25:Antisymmetric relation 1420:Doctoral dissertation 1219:Surface true approach 926: 832:Theoretical arguments 823: 785: 735: 697: 647: 615: 583: 530: 497: 465: 239:⟨A, CP⟩ 231:⟨BP, A⟩ 222: 177: 167:nodes, as defined by 21:Skew-symmetric matrix 1037:-adverbials such as 967:⟨H, C⟩ 964:⟨S, H⟩ 794: 744: 706: 656: 624: 592: 542: 507: 474: 442: 423:Dynamic antisymmetry 414:Dynamic antisymmetry 197:Asymmetric c-command 156:Asymmetric c-command 150:dependency structure 144:. It is disputed by 1225:surface derivations 1018:sentences with the 843:syntactic movements 397:whole of the clause 379:'What did John buy' 275:linguistic typology 268:The universal order 123:syntactic movements 1802:Grammar frameworks 1400:Li, Yafei (2005). 981:syntactic movement 933:Minimalist program 929: 894:Quechuan languages 818: 780: 730: 692: 642: 610: 578: 525: 492: 460: 310:What did John buy? 224: 179: 105:'s 1994 monograph 1797:Generative syntax 1742:978-1-57473-441-6 1449:, pp. 34–36. 1370:Universal Grammar 1072:is indeed found: 1027:perfect aspectual 917:pronominalization 890:relative pronouns 884:(akin to English 538:Kayne notes that 87: 86: 79: 1824: 1783: 1746: 1725: 1698: 1692: 1686: 1683: 1677: 1671: 1660: 1653: 1647: 1641: 1635: 1628: 1622: 1615: 1609: 1606: 1600: 1596: 1590: 1584: 1578: 1572: 1566: 1559: 1553: 1547: 1541: 1535: 1529: 1526: 1520: 1513: 1507: 1501: 1492: 1489: 1483: 1472: 1466: 1464: 1456: 1450: 1444: 1438: 1435: 1429: 1428: 1426: 1412: 1406: 1405: 1397: 1391: 1385: 1357:', Taro thinks ' 1320: 1200: 1182: 1016:Standard Chinese 968: 965: 874:Relative clauses 860:Number agreement 849:direction) are: 827: 825: 824: 819: 789: 787: 786: 781: 739: 737: 736: 731: 701: 699: 698: 693: 651: 649: 648: 643: 619: 617: 616: 611: 587: 585: 584: 579: 534: 532: 531: 526: 501: 499: 498: 493: 469: 467: 466: 461: 369: 351: 340: 244: 240: 236: 232: 111:natural language 103:Richard S. Kayne 99:syntactic theory 82: 75: 71: 68: 62: 42: 41: 34: 1832: 1831: 1827: 1826: 1825: 1823: 1822: 1821: 1787: 1786: 1743: 1722: 1706: 1701: 1693: 1689: 1684: 1680: 1672: 1663: 1654: 1650: 1642: 1638: 1629: 1625: 1616: 1612: 1607: 1603: 1597: 1593: 1585: 1581: 1573: 1569: 1560: 1556: 1548: 1544: 1536: 1532: 1527: 1523: 1514: 1510: 1502: 1495: 1490: 1486: 1473: 1469: 1457: 1453: 1445: 1441: 1436: 1432: 1427:. M.I.T. Press. 1424: 1413: 1409: 1398: 1394: 1386: 1382: 1378: 1361: 1358: 1350: 1345: 1337: 1329: 1322: 1311: 1306: 1300: 1296: 1293: 1292:'Taro thinks ]' 1284: 1276: 1268: 1260: 1252: 1233: 1221: 1205: 1184: 1171: 1163: 1155: 1145: 1137: 1129: 1125: 1111: 1103: 1095: 1085: 1077: 966: 963: 882:complementizers 834: 795: 792: 791: 745: 742: 741: 707: 704: 703: 702:is replaced by 657: 654: 653: 625: 622: 621: 593: 590: 589: 543: 540: 539: 508: 505: 504: 475: 472: 471: 443: 440: 439: 435: 416: 381: 371: 361: 353: 342: 296: 270: 254: 242: 238: 234: 230: 205: 188: 184: 158: 115:branching order 83: 72: 66: 63: 55:help improve it 52: 43: 39: 32: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1830: 1820: 1819: 1814: 1809: 1804: 1799: 1785: 1784: 1747: 1741: 1726: 1721:978-0262611077 1720: 1705: 1702: 1700: 1699: 1687: 1678: 1661: 1648: 1636: 1623: 1610: 1601: 1591: 1579: 1567: 1554: 1542: 1530: 1521: 1508: 1493: 1484: 1467: 1451: 1439: 1430: 1407: 1392: 1379: 1377: 1374: 1348: 1338: 1330: 1323: 1312: 1304: 1301: 1298: 1297: 1277: 1269: 1261: 1253: 1250: 1249: 1232: 1229: 1220: 1217: 1172: 1164: 1156: 1146: 1138: 1130: 1127: 1126: 1104: 1096: 1086: 1078: 1075: 1074: 1008: 1007: 1004: 1001: 998: 995: 992: 970: 969: 957: 956: 921: 920: 897: 871: 857: 854:Hanging topics 833: 830: 817: 814: 811: 808: 805: 802: 799: 779: 776: 773: 770: 767: 764: 761: 758: 755: 752: 749: 729: 726: 723: 720: 717: 714: 711: 691: 688: 685: 682: 679: 676: 673: 670: 667: 664: 661: 641: 638: 635: 632: 629: 609: 606: 603: 600: 597: 577: 574: 571: 568: 565: 562: 559: 556: 553: 550: 547: 524: 521: 518: 515: 512: 491: 488: 485: 482: 479: 459: 456: 453: 450: 447: 434: 431: 415: 412: 407: 406: 362: 354: 343: 332: 331: 327: 326: 324: 323: 322: 295: 292: 269: 266: 253: 250: 204: 201: 186: 182: 169:Tanya Reinhart 157: 154: 85: 84: 46: 44: 37: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1829: 1818: 1815: 1813: 1810: 1808: 1805: 1803: 1800: 1798: 1795: 1794: 1792: 1781: 1777: 1773: 1769: 1765: 1761: 1757: 1753: 1748: 1744: 1738: 1734: 1733: 1727: 1723: 1717: 1714:. MIT Press. 1713: 1708: 1707: 1697:, p. 59. 1696: 1691: 1682: 1676:, p. 57. 1675: 1670: 1668: 1666: 1658: 1652: 1646:, p. 44. 1645: 1640: 1633: 1627: 1620: 1614: 1605: 1595: 1589:, p. 15. 1588: 1583: 1577:, p. 12. 1576: 1571: 1564: 1558: 1551: 1546: 1539: 1534: 1525: 1518: 1512: 1505: 1500: 1498: 1488: 1481: 1477: 1471: 1462: 1455: 1448: 1443: 1434: 1423: 1422: 1419: 1411: 1403: 1396: 1389: 1384: 1380: 1373: 1371: 1365: 1360: 1355: 1352: 1344: 1341: 1336: 1333: 1328: 1325: 1321: 1315: 1310: 1307: 1295: 1290: 1287: 1283: 1280: 1275: 1272: 1267: 1264: 1259: 1256: 1248: 1246: 1245:matrix clause 1241: 1237: 1228: 1226: 1216: 1214: 1210: 1204: 1201: 1195: 1193: 1189: 1183: 1178: 1176: 1170: 1167: 1162: 1159: 1154: 1151: 1150: 1144: 1141: 1136: 1133: 1124: 1121: 1118: 1116: 1110: 1107: 1102: 1099: 1094: 1091: 1090: 1084: 1081: 1073: 1071: 1067: 1063: 1059: 1055: 1053: 1048: 1044: 1040: 1036: 1031: 1028: 1024: 1021: 1017: 1012: 1005: 1002: 999: 996: 993: 990: 989: 988: 986: 982: 977: 975: 962: 961: 960: 954: 953: 952: 950: 945: 942: 938: 934: 925: 918: 914: 910: 906: 902: 898: 895: 891: 887: 883: 879: 875: 872: 869: 865: 861: 858: 855: 852: 851: 850: 846: 844: 839: 829: 812: 809: 806: 803: 800: 774: 771: 768: 762: 756: 753: 750: 724: 721: 718: 715: 712: 686: 683: 680: 674: 668: 665: 662: 636: 633: 630: 604: 601: 598: 572: 569: 566: 560: 554: 551: 548: 536: 519: 516: 513: 486: 483: 480: 454: 451: 448: 430: 428: 427:Phonetic Form 424: 420: 411: 405: 402: 401: 400: 398: 394: 390: 386: 380: 377: 374: 370: 365: 360: 357: 352: 346: 341: 335: 330: 325: 320: 317: 316: 315: 314: 313: 311: 306: 302: 291: 289: 285: 280: 276: 265: 263: 259: 258:first section 249: 246: 227: 221: 217: 215: 211: 200: 198: 194: 191: 176: 172: 170: 166: 162: 153: 151: 147: 143: 139: 135: 130: 128: 124: 120: 116: 112: 108: 104: 101:presented in 100: 96: 92: 81: 78: 70: 60: 56: 50: 47:This article 45: 36: 35: 30: 26: 22: 1758:(1): 41–61. 1755: 1751: 1731: 1711: 1690: 1681: 1651: 1639: 1626: 1613: 1604: 1594: 1582: 1570: 1557: 1552:, p. 7. 1545: 1540:, p. 4. 1533: 1524: 1516: 1511: 1487: 1479: 1470: 1460: 1454: 1442: 1433: 1421: 1417: 1410: 1404:. MIT Press. 1401: 1395: 1383: 1366: 1362: 1356: 1353: 1346: 1342: 1334: 1326: 1316: 1309:discard-even 1308: 1291: 1288: 1285: 1281: 1273: 1265: 1258:discard-even 1257: 1242: 1238: 1234: 1222: 1212: 1208: 1206: 1202: 1196: 1191: 1187: 1185: 1179: 1174: 1168: 1160: 1152: 1148: 1142: 1134: 1122: 1119: 1114: 1112: 1108: 1100: 1092: 1088: 1082: 1069: 1065: 1061: 1057: 1051: 1047:logical form 1038: 1034: 1032: 1022: 1013: 1009: 1006:N] P] V]] C] 978: 971: 958: 946: 944:complement. 930: 913:coordination 885: 867: 847: 835: 537: 436: 417: 408: 396: 392: 384: 382: 378: 375: 372: 366: 358: 347: 336: 328: 318: 309: 297: 271: 262:X-bar theory 255: 247: 228: 225: 213: 209: 206: 196: 195: 192: 180: 159: 131: 119:X-bar theory 106: 95:antisymmetry 94: 88: 73: 64: 48: 1695:Takita 2009 1674:Takita 2009 1644:Takita 2009 1186:* Zhangsan 1043:James Huang 905:dislocation 903:and clitic 419:Andrea Moro 319:wh-movement 301:wh-movement 152:theories). 91:linguistics 67:August 2021 1791:Categories 1587:Kayne 2010 1575:Kayne 2010 1550:Kayne 2010 1538:Kayne 2010 1504:Kayne 2010 1447:Kayne 1994 1388:Kayne 1994 1376:References 1279:omotteiru] 1003:N] P] V]]] 356:kaimashita 214:consistent 138:complement 127:word order 1817:Asymmetry 1780:121661611 1772:0925-8558 1340:omotteiru 1209:zenmeyang 1188:zenmeyang 1149:zenmeyang 1115:zenmeyang 1113:Zhangsan 1089:zenmeyang 1070:zenmeyang 1066:zenmeyang 1058:zenmeyang 1054:-movement 1039:zenmeyang 949:specifier 816:⟩ 798:⟨ 778:⟩ 766:⟨ 760:⟩ 748:⟨ 728:⟩ 710:⟨ 690:⟩ 678:⟨ 672:⟩ 660:⟨ 640:⟩ 628:⟨ 608:⟩ 596:⟨ 576:⟩ 564:⟨ 558:⟩ 546:⟨ 523:⟩ 511:⟨ 490:⟩ 478:⟨ 458:⟩ 446:⟨ 421:proposed 389:auxiliary 161:C-command 134:specifier 1190:xiu che 1143:Zhangsan 1140:Zhangsan 1117:xiu che? 1083:Zhangsan 1080:Zhangsan 1000:N] P]]]] 305:Japanese 284:typology 210:complete 142:movement 1704:Sources 1314:Taro-ga 941:parsing 901:clitics 878:Chinese 334:John-wa 53:Please 1812:Syntax 1778:  1770:  1739:  1718:  1161:repair 1135:  1101:repair 997:N]]]]] 985:Basque 359:bought 345:nani-o 288:German 185:and AP 1776:S2CID 1425:(PDF) 1343:think 1317:Taro- 1282:think 1263:shita 994:]]]]] 991:]]]]] 974:X-bar 888:) or 348:what- 337:John- 279:heads 97:is a 1768:ISSN 1737:ISBN 1716:ISBN 1347:-sae 1303:-sae 1255:-sae 1106:che? 955:S H 937:head 886:that 620:and 404:C ka 212:and 165:tree 136:and 1760:doi 1332:to] 1327:did 1319:NOM 1271:to] 1266:did 1169:car 1166:che 1158:xiu 1153:how 1128:(b) 1109:car 1098:xiu 1093:how 1076:(a) 790:to 393:did 350:ACC 339:TOP 303:in 89:In 57:to 1793:: 1774:. 1766:. 1756:18 1754:. 1664:^ 1599:5. 1496:^ 1335:C‍ 1299:b. 1274:C‍ 1251:a. 1213:le 1199:PF 1192:le 1181:PF 1175:le 1062:le 1052:wh 1035:wh 1023:le 911:, 907:, 896:.) 385:ka 364:ka 245:. 237:, 233:, 181:AP 129:. 93:, 1782:. 1762:: 1745:. 1724:. 1506:. 1390:. 1349:i 1305:i 1194:? 1177:? 1132:* 919:. 813:C 810:, 807:H 804:, 801:S 775:C 772:, 769:H 763:, 757:H 754:, 751:S 725:C 722:, 719:H 716:, 713:S 687:C 684:, 681:H 675:, 669:H 666:, 663:S 637:C 634:, 631:H 605:H 602:, 599:S 573:C 570:, 567:H 561:, 555:H 552:, 549:S 520:H 517:, 514:S 487:C 484:, 481:H 455:C 452:, 449:H 368:Q 321:→ 187:2 183:1 80:) 74:( 69:) 65:( 51:. 31:.

Index

Skew-symmetric matrix
Antisymmetric relation
Antisymmetric (disambiguation)
help improve it
make it understandable to non-experts
Learn how and when to remove this message
linguistics
syntactic theory
Richard S. Kayne
natural language
branching order
X-bar theory
syntactic movements
word order
specifier
complement
movement
constituency structure
dependency structure
C-command
tree
Tanya Reinhart


first section
X-bar theory
linguistic typology
heads
typology
German

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.