Knowledge

Modes of religiosity

Source 📝

75:
larger, more complex civilizations. Similarly, historians and biologists have utilized DMR theory to help explain why some religions separate into sects and how reformations may occur. However, the theory has been challenged by some scholars on theoretical grounds and faced criticisms from some ethnographers and historians for suggesting too strong of a division between imagistic and doctrinal modes.
67:. Later formulations of the theory also emphasised the different forms of group cohesion that are generated by the two modes, with imagistic rituals promoting intense, relational bonds with the other ritual participants and doctrinal rituals promoting more diffuse, categorical bonds with larger communities who share the same identity markers. 74:
global ethnographic database and the anticipated clustering of rituals around imagistic and doctrinal modes was confirmed. Ethnographic and historical examinations of the theory have been broadly positive and archaeologists have drawn upon DMR to explain the transition from small-scale societies to
127:
and scripts of general knowledge. In contrast to the imagistic mode, these routinized rituals tend to produce less intense group identification, which serves to promote trust and cooperation but not extreme self-sacrifice. DMR posits that the historical transition from small-scale societies to the
78:
Although the DMR theory developed out of research on religious groups, more recent research has found evidence that the ritual dynamics described apply outside of the religious domain, including amongst football fans and armed militias, and that it may therefore serve as a more general theory of
99:. DMR posits that fusion with other group members will also motivate the individual to act out extreme forms of altruism, especially when the group is threatened. Therefore, the imagistic mode of religiosity prevails when a group’s survival depends on extremely high levels of cohesion. 51:
The theory posits that these differing ritual patterns promote the transmission of religious traditions by exploiting core memory processes. Imagistic rituals arouse strong emotion and generate vivid,
48:
mode is characterized by frequently performed, low arousal rituals (e.g. daily recitations of sacred texts) and is associated with larger inclusive communities, as found in the major world religions.
311:
Mithen, S. (2004). "From Ohalo to Çatalhöyük: The development of religiosity during the early prehistory of Western Asia, 20,000–7000 BC". In Whitehouse, H.; Martin, L.H. (eds.).
223: 87:
The imagistic mode of religiosity involves collective rituals that are infrequent and highly emotional. Examples of these types of rituals include various initiation rites and
707:
Whitehouse, H.; Jong, J.; Buhrmester, M.D.; Gómez, Á.; Bastian, B.; Kavanagh, C.M.; Newson, M.; Matthews, M.; Lanman, J.A.; McKay, R.; Gavrilets, S. (2017).
371:
Pyysiainen, I. (2004). "Corrupt Doctrine and Doctrinal Revival: On the Nature and Limits of the Modes Theory.". In Whitehouse, H.; Martin, L.H. (eds.).
326:
Johnson, K. (2004). "Primary emergence of the doctrinal mode of religiosity in prehistoric southwestern Iran.". In Whitehouse, H.; Martin, L.H. (eds.).
95:
system, resulting in detailed autobiographical memories. These dysphoric rituals can produce an extreme form of cohesion with the group, known as
621:
Swann, W.B.; Jensen, J.; Gomez, A.; Whitehouse, H.; Bastian, B. (2012). "When Group Membership Gets Personal: A theory of identity fusion".
413:
Schjoedt, Uffe; Sørensen, Jesper; Nielbo, Kristoffer Laigaard; Xygalatas, Dimitris; Mitkidis, Panagiotis; Bulbulia, Joseph (2013-02-01).
414: 107:
The doctrinal mode of religiosity refers to collective rituals that are frequent, usually routinized, and generate relatively little
606:
Whitehouse, H. (2004). "Rites of Terror: emotion, metaphor, and memory in Melanesian initiation cults.". In Corrigan, J. (ed.).
59:, while doctrinal rituals repetitive nature means that rather than individual events the experiences over time are stored in 20:
is intended to explain how religions are created, transmitted, and changed. DMR theory was first developed by anthropologist
824: 387: 203: 178: 153: 508: 481: 397: 765: 341:
Gragg, D. (2004). "Old and New in Roman Religion: A Cognitive Account.". In Whitehouse, H.; Martin, L.H. (eds.).
467: 356:
Hinde, R. (2005). "Modes Theory: Some Theoretical Considerations.". In Whitehouse, H.; McCauley, R.N. (eds.).
819: 584: 829: 71: 128:
invention of agriculture brought about the need for large-scale cooperation and collective identity.
800: 296:
Whitehouse, H.; Hodder, I. (2010). "Modes of religiosity at Çatalhöyük". In Hodder, I. (ed.).
720: 767:
The Role of Ritual in the Evolution of Social Complexity: Five predictions and a drum roll
8: 524:
Whitehouse, Harvey; McQuinn, Brian; Buhrmester, Michael; Swann, William B. (2014-12-16).
124: 108: 91:. The often dysphoric and highly emotional nature of these types of rituals activate the 28:. The theory proposes that religions tend to coalesce around two divergent modes, termed 724: 44:
rites) and is associated with small scale, exclusive religious groups. In contrast the
788: 741: 708: 684: 657: 560: 525: 445: 274: 243: 123:
systems are thought to be activated and function similarly to organizing other general
746: 689: 638: 565: 547: 504: 477: 463: 437: 393: 278: 199: 174: 149: 60: 21: 449: 247: 774: 736: 728: 679: 669: 630: 555: 537: 429: 270: 235: 52: 25: 261:
Atkinson, Q.D.; Whitehouse, H. (2011). "The cultural morphospace of ritual form".
498: 471: 433: 120: 96: 92: 88: 64: 56: 116: 112: 813: 551: 441: 282: 674: 542: 40:
mode is characterized by infrequently performed, high arousal rituals (e.g.
779: 750: 693: 642: 569: 146:
Inside the cult: religious innovation and transmission in Papua New Guinea
358:
Mind and Religion: Psychological and Cognitive Foundations of Religiosity
709:"The evolution of extreme cooperation via shared dysphoric experiences" 70:
DMR theory was tested empirically against data on 645 rituals from the
41: 732: 500:
Mind and Religion: Psychological and Cognitive Foundations of Religion
634: 373:
Theorizing Religions Past: historical and archaeological perspectives
343:
Theorizing Religions Past: historical and archaeological perspectives
328:
Theorizing Religions Past: Historical and Archaeological Perspectives
313:
Theorizing Religions Past: Historical and Archaeological Perspectives
298:
Religion in the Emergence of Civilization: Çatalhöyük as a Case Study
389:
Bringing Ritual to Mind: Psychological Foundations of Cultural Forms
239: 36:, which are distinguished primarily by their ritual practices. The 196:
Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission
656:
Whitehouse, H.; McQuinn, B.; Burhmester, M.; Swann, W.B. (2014).
655: 523: 473:
Ritual and memory: toward a comparative anthropology of religion
412: 706: 658:"Brothers in Arms: Libyan revolutionaries bond like Family" 526:"Brothers in arms: Libyan revolutionaries bond like family" 224:"The Ties That Bind Us: Ritual, Fusion, and Identification" 111:. Examples of this type of collective ritual would include 620: 300:. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 122–145. 119:. Due to the repetitive nature of these types of rituals, 763: 415:"Cognitive resource depletion in religious interactions" 497:
Whitehouse, Harvey; McCauley, Robert N. (2005-06-02).
386:
McCauley, Robert N.; Lawson, E. Thomas (2002-08-15).
610:. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 703–715. 608:
Religion and Emotion: Approaches and Interpretations
315:. Walnut Creek, CA.: AltaMira Press. pp. 17–43. 330:. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. pp. 45–66. 171:
Arguments and icons: divergent modes of religiosity
764:Whitehouse, H.; Francois, P.; Turchin, P. (2015). 496: 260: 462: 811: 295: 221: 662:Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 530:Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 385: 137: 605: 370: 349: 193: 168: 143: 778: 740: 683: 673: 559: 541: 24:following his ethnographic fieldwork in 325: 812: 310: 198:. Walnut Creek, CA.: Alta Mira Press. 585:"Social evolution: The ritual animal" 582: 355: 340: 222:Whitehouse, H.; Lanman, J.A. (2014). 217: 215: 18:divergent modes of religiosity (DMR) 375:. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. 360:. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. 345:. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. 173:. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 13: 275:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.002 14: 841: 773:. Vol. 6. pp. 199–210. 212: 102: 82: 757: 700: 649: 614: 599: 576: 517: 490: 456: 406: 379: 364: 422:Religion, Brain & Behavior 392:. Cambridge University Press. 334: 319: 304: 289: 254: 187: 162: 1: 131: 434:10.1080/2153599X.2012.736714 263:Evolution and Human Behavior 79:ritual and social cohesion. 7: 148:. Oxford: Clarendon Press. 144:Whitehouse, Harvey (1995). 10: 846: 825:Psychological anthropology 583:Jones, Dan (23 Jan 2013). 675:10.1073/pnas.1416284111 543:10.1073/pnas.1416284111 194:Whitehouse, H. (2004). 169:Whitehouse, H. (2000). 780:10.21237/C7clio6229624 623:Psychological Review 228:Current Anthropology 820:Religious practices 725:2017NatSR...744292W 668:(50): 17783–17785. 536:(50): 17783–17785. 503:. Rowman Altamira. 713:Scientific Reports 476:. AltaMira Press. 464:Whitehouse, Harvey 830:Social psychology 733:10.1038/srep44292 65:semantic memories 57:episodic memories 22:Harvey Whitehouse 837: 805: 804: 798: 794: 792: 784: 782: 772: 761: 755: 754: 744: 704: 698: 697: 687: 677: 653: 647: 646: 635:10.1037/a0028589 618: 612: 611: 603: 597: 596: 594: 592: 580: 574: 573: 563: 545: 521: 515: 514: 494: 488: 487: 460: 454: 453: 419: 410: 404: 403: 383: 377: 376: 368: 362: 361: 353: 347: 346: 338: 332: 331: 323: 317: 316: 308: 302: 301: 293: 287: 286: 258: 252: 251: 219: 210: 209: 191: 185: 184: 166: 160: 159: 141: 89:rites of passage 26:Papua New Guinea 845: 844: 840: 839: 838: 836: 835: 834: 810: 809: 808: 796: 795: 786: 785: 770: 762: 758: 705: 701: 654: 650: 619: 615: 604: 600: 590: 588: 581: 577: 522: 518: 511: 495: 491: 484: 461: 457: 417: 411: 407: 400: 384: 380: 369: 365: 354: 350: 339: 335: 324: 320: 309: 305: 294: 290: 259: 255: 220: 213: 206: 192: 188: 181: 167: 163: 156: 142: 138: 134: 121:semantic memory 105: 97:identity fusion 93:episodic memory 85: 12: 11: 5: 843: 833: 832: 827: 822: 807: 806: 756: 699: 648: 629:(3): 441–456. 613: 598: 575: 516: 509: 489: 482: 470:(2004-01-01). 468:Laidlaw, James 455: 405: 398: 378: 363: 348: 333: 318: 303: 288: 253: 240:10.1086/678698 234:(6): 674–695. 211: 205:978-0759106154 204: 186: 180:978-0198234159 179: 161: 155:978-0198279815 154: 135: 133: 130: 117:call to prayer 113:Holy Communion 104: 103:Doctrinal mode 101: 84: 83:Imagistic mode 81: 16:The theory of 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 842: 831: 828: 826: 823: 821: 818: 817: 815: 802: 790: 781: 776: 769: 768: 760: 752: 748: 743: 738: 734: 730: 726: 722: 718: 714: 710: 703: 695: 691: 686: 681: 676: 671: 667: 663: 659: 652: 644: 640: 636: 632: 628: 624: 617: 609: 602: 586: 579: 571: 567: 562: 557: 553: 549: 544: 539: 535: 531: 527: 520: 512: 510:9780759114838 506: 502: 501: 493: 485: 483:9780759106161 479: 475: 474: 469: 465: 459: 451: 447: 443: 439: 435: 431: 427: 423: 416: 409: 401: 399:9780521016292 395: 391: 390: 382: 374: 367: 359: 352: 344: 337: 329: 322: 314: 307: 299: 292: 284: 280: 276: 272: 268: 264: 257: 249: 245: 241: 237: 233: 229: 225: 218: 216: 207: 201: 197: 190: 182: 176: 172: 165: 157: 151: 147: 140: 136: 129: 126: 122: 118: 114: 110: 100: 98: 94: 90: 80: 76: 73: 68: 66: 62: 58: 54: 49: 47: 43: 39: 35: 31: 27: 23: 19: 766: 759: 716: 712: 702: 665: 661: 651: 626: 622: 616: 607: 601: 589:. Retrieved 578: 533: 529: 519: 499: 492: 472: 458: 428:(1): 39–55. 425: 421: 408: 388: 381: 372: 366: 357: 351: 342: 336: 327: 321: 312: 306: 297: 291: 269:(1): 50–62. 266: 262: 256: 231: 227: 195: 189: 170: 164: 145: 139: 106: 86: 77: 69: 50: 45: 37: 33: 29: 17: 15: 797:|work= 814:Categories 132:References 61:procedural 42:initiation 799:ignored ( 789:cite book 719:: 44292. 552:0027-8424 442:2153-599X 283:1090-5138 53:flashbulb 46:doctrinal 38:imagistic 34:doctrinal 30:imagistic 751:28290499 694:25385591 643:22642548 587:. Nature 570:25385591 450:40315884 248:45622337 742:5349572 721:Bibcode 685:4273349 591:3 April 561:4273349 125:schemas 749:  739:  692:  682:  641:  568:  558:  550:  507:  480:  448:  440:  396:  281:  246:  202:  177:  152:  109:affect 55:like, 771:(PDF) 446:S2CID 418:(PDF) 244:S2CID 801:help 747:PMID 690:PMID 639:PMID 593:2017 566:PMID 548:ISSN 505:ISBN 478:ISBN 438:ISSN 394:ISBN 279:ISSN 200:ISBN 175:ISBN 150:ISBN 115:and 72:HRAF 63:and 32:and 775:doi 737:PMC 729:doi 680:PMC 670:doi 666:111 631:doi 627:119 556:PMC 538:doi 534:111 430:doi 271:doi 236:doi 816:: 793:: 791:}} 787:{{ 745:. 735:. 727:. 715:. 711:. 688:. 678:. 664:. 660:. 637:. 625:. 564:. 554:. 546:. 532:. 528:. 466:; 444:. 436:. 424:. 420:. 277:. 267:32 265:. 242:. 232:55 230:. 226:. 214:^ 803:) 783:. 777:: 753:. 731:: 723:: 717:7 696:. 672:: 645:. 633:: 595:. 572:. 540:: 513:. 486:. 452:. 432:: 426:3 402:. 285:. 273:: 250:. 238:: 208:. 183:. 158:.

Index

Harvey Whitehouse
Papua New Guinea
initiation
flashbulb
episodic memories
procedural
semantic memories
HRAF
rites of passage
episodic memory
identity fusion
affect
Holy Communion
call to prayer
semantic memory
schemas
ISBN
978-0198279815
ISBN
978-0198234159
ISBN
978-0759106154


"The Ties That Bind Us: Ritual, Fusion, and Identification"
doi
10.1086/678698
S2CID
45622337
doi

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.