Knowledge

Heuristic-systematic model of information processing

Source đź“ť

376:
hypothesize that systematic processing requires and uses cognitive capacity, while heuristic processing makes low cognitive demands. Furthermore, both HSM and ELM assume that "capacity and motivation are important determinants of systematic process" which results in biased modes of processing (p. 327). With heuristic processing, there is less need to process information and cognitively in comparison to systematic processing. Heuristic processing occurs when people simply form immediate decisions and conclusions based on the information available versus analytical processing of information given that obviously requires more cognition. Heuristic processing as defined by HSM, illustrates that people can formulate decisions utilizing basic rules such as "experts' statements can be trusted" and "consensus implies correctness" to establish validity within messages (p. 327). Therefore, individuals who process messages through heuristic processing routes of persuasion, likely formulate decisions based on experts’ opinion and what the consensus believes opposed to fully processing the message in its entirety.
222:, which stated that information processing outcomes were the result of interaction between a fast, associative information-processing mode based on low-effort heuristics, and a slow, rule-based information processing mode based on high-effort systematic reasoning. Wathen and Burkell proposed (but did not test) that if an individual determines that an online source does not meet an appropriate level of credibility at any one stage, then he or she will leave the site without further evaluation. They theorized that this “easy to discard” behavior was indicative of information-rich environments, where the assumption is that many other potential sources of information exist, and spending too much time on any one source is potentially wasteful. 384:
not. Another assumption by Chaiken and her colleagues is that systematic processing does in fact provide people with more judgment relevant information in comparison to heuristic processing of information, which does not account for any weaknesses in expert subject matter material. Therefore, while systematic processing may be prevalent within many social environments, HSM, unlike its model counterpart ELM, does illustrate "the possibility that heuristic processing can exert a significant and independent influence on persuasion" (p 329).
333:. Chaiken assumes that the primary processing goal of accuracy-motivated recipients is to assess the validity of persuasive messages, and that both heuristic and systematic processing can serve this objective. Other motives beyond the validity-seeking persuasion context were identified by Chaiken and colleagues (1989) who proposed an expanded model that posits two additional motives that heuristic and systematic processing can serve: defense-motivation and impression-motivation. 368:
the level of involvement in the topic or issue. High involvement or elaboration increases central route processing especially when motivation and ability in the message exists. Therefore, low involvement increases peripheral route processing when motivation and ability conditions of persuasion do not exist. However, if the topic or idea is irrelevant to the individual, then the message takes the peripheral route.
101:". This principle stated that in the interest of economy, the mind would often process with the least amount of effort (i.e., use a heuristic), and for more detailed information processing would use more effortful (systematic) processing. This was the major difference when compared with the ELM, which described the two different ways information was processed, through central and/or peripheral processing. 364:, which is also a dual-processing model discussing two main paths to persuasion. The ELM discusses the two routes as "central" route processing and "peripheral" route processing. ELM's central processing has been likened to systematic processing in HSM, while peripheral processing is similar to HSM's heuristic processing. These two routes of processing define related theories behind attitude change. 136:
refers to the relevancy of the memory to the judgmental task. Due to the use of knowledge structures, a person using heuristic information processing is likely to agree with messages delivered by experts, or messages that are endorsed by others, without fully processing the semantic content of the message. In comparison to systematic processing, heuristic processing entails judging the
272: 163:
conclusion. Systematic views of persuasion emphasize detailed processing of message content and the role of message-based cognitions in mediating opinion change. While recipients utilizing systematic processing rely heavily on message content, source characteristics and other non-content may supplement the recipients’ assessment of validity in the persuasive message.
258:-laden and when men were uncertain about the probabilities for cure and side effects. There was a trend for decreased systematic processing when the expert opinion heuristic was used. Findings were consistent with the heuristic-systematic processing model and suggest that this model has utility for future research in applied decision making about health issues. 82:, and Kelley noted that a sense of "rightness" accompanies holding opinions similar to the opinions of others. In 1987, Holtz and Miller reaffirmed this line of thought by noting, "When other people are perceived to hold similar attitudes, one's confidence in the validity of one's own attitude is increased." 194:
use a systematic processing strategy. Reliability concerns are influenced by the level of the recipient's issue-involvement or response-involvement. When the recipient views their judgment as being less consequential, they will likely place greater value on economic concerns than reliability concerns.
135:
Heuristic processing is governed by availability, accessibility, and applicability. Availability refers to the knowledge structure, or heuristic, being stored in memory for future use. Accessibility of the heuristic applies to the ability to retrieve the memory for use. Applicability of the heuristic
383:
HSM postulates that heuristic and systematic processing can each influence both "independent" and "interdependent" effects on decision making by occurring simultaneously (p. 328). Unlike HSM, ELM does not postulate whether central route processing and peripheral route processing can co-occur or
162:
content of the message. Recipients developing attitudes from a systematic basis exert considerable cognitive effort and actively attempt to comprehend and evaluate the message's arguments. When processing systematically, recipients also attempt to assess their validity as it relates to the message's
104:
The developer and main researcher of the HSM was Shelly Chaiken. Under her direction, the HSM has undergone several major revisions. As she noted in 1980 and 1987, the model specified the two modes of heuristic and systematic processing. Then, Chaiken et al. noted in 1989 that the model was extended
371:
HSM specifically examines validity seeking persuasion settings concerning people's motivations within the social environment. The limitation of HSM exists in the inability to define the specific motivations of persuasion, which is why Chaiken expanded HSM to illustrate that heuristic and systematic
367:
In ELM, the central route is reflective and requires a willingness to process and think about the message. The peripheral route occurs when attitudes are formed without extensive thought, but more from mental shortcuts, credibility, and appearance cues. The route of persuasion processing depends on
193:
When economic concerns are predominant, the recipient will likely use heuristic processing to form a judgement about the persuasive argument. Conversely, when reliability concerns are predominant (i.e., recipients perceive significant importance in accurately judging an argument), they will likely
43:
or systematically. Systematic processing entails careful and deliberative processing of a message, while heuristic processing entails the use of simplifying decision rules or 'heuristics' to quickly assess the message content. The guiding belief with this model is that individuals are more apt to
379:
This leads to another similarity between HSM and ELM, as attitudes and opinions developed through heuristic processing will tend to be "less stable, less resistant to counter-propaganda, and less predictive of behavior" in comparison to attitudes and opinions formed through detailed information
375:
Major assumptions exist with both HSM and ELM, which is why both models have generated debate and are often misconstrued. Systematic processing assumes that persuasion has occurred via the recipient's understanding and cognitive elaboration of the persuasive argument. In addition, researchers
140:
of messages by relying more on accessible context information, such as the identity of the source or other non-content cues. Thus, heuristic views de-emphasize detailed information evaluation and focus on the role of simple rules or cognitive heuristics in mediating persuasion.
144:
Individuals may be more likely to use heuristic processing when an issue is less personally important to them (they have low “issue involvement”) or when they believe their judgment will not have significant impacts on themselves (low “response involvement”).
249:
Men's use of the expert opinion heuristic was related to men's verbal reports of decisional uncertainty and having a positive orientation to their doctor and medical care; a desire for greater involvement in decision making was predicted by a high internal
157:
and message content, which may exert stronger impact on persuasion, when determining message validity. Judgments developed from systematic processing rely heavily on in-depth treatment of judgment-relevant information and respond accordingly to the
601:
Eagly, A.H. & Chaiken, S. (1993). Process theories of attitude formation and change: The elaboration likelihood and heuristic-systematic models. In A.H. Eagly & S. Chaiken, (Eds.), The psychology of attitudes. Orlando: Harcourt Brace: pp.
105:
to specify the psychological conditions for triggering the modes of processing in terms of the discrepancy between actual and desired subjective confidence. In 1986, Chaiken, and others, updated the model to include underlying
213:
In a 2002 study by Wathen & Burkell, they proposed a theory that separated the evaluation process into distinct segments. In the theory, the process began with low-effort examinations of peripheral cues (e.g., appearance,
372:
processing can "serve defense-motivation, the desire to form or defend particular attitudinal positions, and impression- motivation, the desire to form or hold socially acceptable attitudinal positions" (p. 326).
446:
Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic Versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source Versus Message Cues in Persuasion. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 39(5), 752-766. Retrieved from SocINDEX
183:
Message recipients using heuristic processing may sometimes choose to accept message conclusions they would otherwise have rejected, or vice versa, had they invested more time and effort to scrutinize the message.
171:
Both heuristic and systematic processes may occur independently. It is also possible for both to occur simultaneously in an additive fashion or in a way that the judgmental implications of one process lend a
176:
nature to the other. The heuristic-systematic model includes the hypothesis that attitudes developed or changed by utilizing heuristic processing alone will likely be less stable, less resistant to
218:, organization, and source reputation) then continued to a more high-effort analysis of the content of the information source. The proposed research also drew on social psychological theories of 671:
Wathen, C. N., & Burkell, J. (2002). Believe it or not: Factors influencing credibility on the Web. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 53(2), 134–144
290: 237:
investigated the utility of the heuristic-systematic processing model as a framework for the investigation of patient decision making. A total of 111 men diagnosed with localized
681:
Steginga, Suzanne K.; Occhipinti, Stefano (2004). "The Application of the Heuristic-Systematic Processing Model to Treatment Decision Making about Prostate Cancer".
530:
Chen, S., Duckworth, K., & Chaiken, S. (1999). Motivated Heuristic and Systematic Processing. Psychological Inquiry, 10(1), 44. Retrieved from SocINDEX database
153:
Systematic processing involves comprehensive and analytic, cognitive processing of judgment-relevant information. The systematic approach values source
494:
Petty, R.E. & Cacioppo, J.T. (1986), Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York; Springer-Verlag
472: 71:’s (1950) statement that incorrect or improper attitudes are generally maladaptive and can have deleterious behavioral and affective consequences. 512:
Dillard, James, and Michael Pfau. The persuasion handbook: developments in theory and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, 2002. Print.
485:
Albarracin, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). The handbook of attitudes. Mahwah, N.J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
348:
should be treated as complementary models to create a dual-processing framework for use in future research for understanding a variety of
254:
of health control. Trends were observed for systematic information processing to increase when the heuristic strategy used was negatively
67:
processed each element of a message. One of the early guiding principles of underlying motivations of persuasive communications came from
97:
when presented with persuasive information. Based on this thought, early assumptions said people were at least partially guided by the "
40: 457:
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. (1989). "Heuristic and systematic processing within and beyond the persuasion context".
44:
minimize their use of cognitive resources (i.e., to rely on heuristics), thus affecting the intake and processing of messages.
219: 55:, or ELM. Both models were predominantly developed in the early- to mid-1980s and share many of the same concepts and ideas. 180:, and will be less predictive of subsequent behavior than attitudes developed or changed utilizing systematic processing. 63:
Early research investigating how people process persuasive messaging focused mainly on cognitive theories and the way the
94: 408: 246: 308: 403: 47:
HSM predicts that processing type will influence the extent to which a person is persuaded or exhibits lasting
759: 503:
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology . New York: Guilford Press.
398: 361: 345: 52: 282: 190:
affects persuasion under conditions of low, but not high, issue-involvement and response-involvement.
764: 749: 98: 739: 242: 154: 230: 723:
Griffin, E. A. (2006). A first look at communication theory (6th ed.) Boston, MA; McGraw Hill
340:
Impression-motivation is the desire to form or hold socially acceptable attitudinal positions.
120:
processing uses judgmental rules known as knowledge structures that are learned and stored in
466: 86: 48: 202:
Research into information processing, especially in persuasive messaging, can be applied in
638: 8: 754: 744: 393: 255: 234: 629: 128:
effort on the part of the recipient. Heuristic processing is related to the concept of "
706: 557: 321:
Originally the heuristic-systematic model was developed to apply to "validity seeking"
187: 698: 651: 643: 710: 337:
Defense-motivation is the desire to form or defend particular attitudinal positions.
690: 633: 625: 549: 349: 251: 226: 225:
The HSM has also been applied in medical decision-making contexts. A 2004 study by
137: 238: 177: 68: 25: 733: 694: 647: 245:
and self-report measures. The results showed: "Most men (68%) preferred that
702: 655: 124:. The heuristic approach offers an economic advantage by requiring minimal 79: 75: 456: 203: 129: 344:
Contrary to previous viewpoints, the heuristic-systematic model and the
680: 561: 322: 106: 29: 89:. This principle reflected widespread notions that people use limited 159: 125: 117: 90: 553: 540:
Simon, Herbert A. (1955). "A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice".
326: 207: 36: 247:
decision making be shared equally between them and their doctor.
215: 121: 360:
A major criticism of HSM is that the model closely relates to
166: 459:
In J. S. Veleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended Thought
613: 330: 325:
contexts in which peoples' primary motivation is to attain
173: 64: 28:
that attempts to explain how people receive and process
612:
Gigerenzer, Gerd; Gaissmaier, Wolfgang (2011-01-10).
611: 85:
Another concept that contributed to the HSM was the
18:
heuristic-systematic model of information processing
93:resources, or use an "economy-minded" approach to 731: 261: 206:. For instance, HSM has been used in Internet 35:The model states that individuals can process 471:: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list ( 380:within systematic processing (p. 327). 167:Choosing systematic or heuristic processing 667: 665: 637: 442: 440: 438: 436: 434: 432: 430: 428: 426: 424: 309:Learn how and when to remove this message 597: 595: 593: 591: 233:and the School of Applied Psychology at 197: 148: 589: 587: 585: 583: 581: 579: 577: 575: 573: 571: 497: 112: 732: 662: 526: 524: 522: 520: 518: 506: 421: 539: 409:Extended transportation-imagery model 717: 568: 488: 265: 229:, PhD, and Stefano Occhipinti, PhD, 51:change. HSM is quite similar to the 674: 630:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 515: 479: 329:attitudes that align with relevant 13: 542:The Quarterly Journal of Economics 24:) is a widely recognized model by 14: 776: 270: 605: 404:Extended parallel process model 639:11858/00-001M-0000-0024-F16D-5 533: 461:. New York: Guilford: 212–252. 450: 1: 414: 355: 399:Elaboration likelihood model 346:elaboration likelihood model 262:Direction of future research 53:elaboration likelihood model 7: 618:Annual Review of Psychology 614:"Heuristic Decision Making" 387: 10: 781: 58: 99:principle of least effort 695:10.1177/0272989X04271044 243:verbal protocol analysis 231:Queensland Cancer Fund 95:information processing 198:Practical application 149:Systematic processing 87:sufficiency principle 760:Psychological models 241:were assessed using 113:Heuristic processing 39:in one of two ways: 394:Bounded rationality 235:Griffith University 227:Suzanne K. Steginga 289:You can assist by 188:Source credibility 319: 318: 311: 772: 765:Mental processes 750:1980s neologisms 724: 721: 715: 714: 683:Med Decis Making 678: 672: 669: 660: 659: 641: 609: 603: 599: 566: 565: 537: 531: 528: 513: 510: 504: 501: 495: 492: 486: 483: 477: 476: 470: 462: 454: 448: 444: 350:social influence 314: 307: 303: 300: 294: 274: 273: 266: 210:considerations. 178:counterarguments 780: 779: 775: 774: 773: 771: 770: 769: 740:Attitude change 730: 729: 728: 727: 722: 718: 679: 675: 670: 663: 610: 606: 600: 569: 554:10.2307/1884852 538: 534: 529: 516: 511: 507: 502: 498: 493: 489: 484: 480: 464: 463: 455: 451: 445: 422: 417: 390: 358: 315: 304: 298: 295: 288: 275: 271: 264: 239:prostate cancer 220:dual-processing 200: 169: 151: 115: 61: 12: 11: 5: 778: 768: 767: 762: 757: 752: 747: 742: 726: 725: 716: 689:(6): 573–583. 673: 661: 624:(1): 451–482. 604: 567: 532: 514: 505: 496: 487: 478: 449: 419: 418: 416: 413: 412: 411: 406: 401: 396: 389: 386: 357: 354: 342: 341: 338: 317: 316: 278: 276: 269: 263: 260: 199: 196: 168: 165: 150: 147: 114: 111: 69:Leon Festinger 60: 57: 26:Shelly Chaiken 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 777: 766: 763: 761: 758: 756: 753: 751: 748: 746: 743: 741: 738: 737: 735: 720: 712: 708: 704: 700: 696: 692: 688: 684: 677: 668: 666: 657: 653: 649: 645: 640: 635: 631: 627: 623: 619: 615: 608: 598: 596: 594: 592: 590: 588: 586: 584: 582: 580: 578: 576: 574: 572: 563: 559: 555: 551: 548:(1): 99–118. 547: 543: 536: 527: 525: 523: 521: 519: 509: 500: 491: 482: 474: 468: 460: 453: 443: 441: 439: 437: 435: 433: 431: 429: 427: 425: 420: 410: 407: 405: 402: 400: 397: 395: 392: 391: 385: 381: 377: 373: 369: 365: 363: 353: 351: 347: 339: 336: 335: 334: 332: 328: 324: 313: 310: 302: 292: 286: 284: 279:This article 277: 268: 267: 259: 257: 253: 248: 244: 240: 236: 232: 228: 223: 221: 217: 211: 209: 205: 195: 191: 189: 185: 181: 179: 175: 164: 161: 156: 146: 142: 139: 133: 131: 127: 123: 119: 110: 108: 102: 100: 96: 92: 88: 83: 81: 77: 72: 70: 66: 56: 54: 50: 45: 42: 41:heuristically 38: 33: 31: 27: 23: 19: 719: 686: 682: 676: 621: 617: 607: 545: 541: 535: 508: 499: 490: 481: 467:cite journal 458: 452: 382: 378: 374: 370: 366: 359: 343: 320: 305: 296: 283:copy editing 281:may require 280: 224: 212: 201: 192: 186: 182: 170: 152: 143: 134: 116: 103: 84: 73: 62: 46: 34: 21: 17: 15: 352:phenomena. 204:advertising 155:reliability 130:satisficing 107:motivations 32:messages. 755:Persuasion 745:Heuristics 734:Categories 415:References 356:Criticisms 323:persuasion 291:editing it 30:persuasive 648:0066-4308 447:database. 299:June 2024 126:cognitive 118:Heuristic 91:cognitive 74:In 1953, 711:36170137 703:15534339 656:21126183 602:303-350. 388:See also 327:accurate 285:for tone 160:semantic 138:validity 49:attitude 37:messages 562:1884852 208:webpage 76:Hovland 59:History 709:  701:  654:  646:  560:  256:affect 216:design 122:memory 707:S2CID 558:JSTOR 331:facts 252:locus 80:Janis 699:PMID 652:PMID 644:ISSN 473:link 174:bias 65:mind 16:The 691:doi 634:hdl 626:doi 550:doi 362:ELM 132:." 22:HSM 736:: 705:. 697:. 687:24 685:. 664:^ 650:. 642:. 632:. 622:62 620:. 616:. 570:^ 556:. 546:69 544:. 517:^ 469:}} 465:{{ 423:^ 109:. 78:, 713:. 693:: 658:. 636:: 628:: 564:. 552:: 475:) 312:) 306:( 301:) 297:( 293:. 287:. 20:(

Index

Shelly Chaiken
persuasive
messages
heuristically
attitude
elaboration likelihood model
mind
Leon Festinger
Hovland
Janis
sufficiency principle
cognitive
information processing
principle of least effort
motivations
Heuristic
memory
cognitive
satisficing
validity
reliability
semantic
bias
counterarguments
Source credibility
advertising
webpage
design
dual-processing
Suzanne K. Steginga

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑