Knowledge

Transfer of learning

Source 📝

70:. They explored how individuals would transfer learning in one context to another similar context and how "improvement in one mental function" could influence a related one. Their theory implied that transfer of learning depends on how similar the learning task and transfer tasks are, or where "identical elements are concerned in the influencing and influenced function", now known as the 131:, or basic units of knowledge, in long-term memory. When new information enters the working memory, long-term memory is searched for associations which combine with the new information in working memory. The associations reinforce the new information and help assign meaning to it. Learning that takes place in varying contexts can create more links and encourage 85:
expected little transfer of learning. Guthrie recommended studying in the exact conditions in which one would be tested, because of his view that "we learn what we do in the presence of specific stimuli". The expectation is that training in conditions as similar as possible to those in which learners
160:
Learners can increase transfer through effective practice and by mindfully abstracting knowledge. Abstraction is the process of examining our experiences for similarities. Methods for abstracting knowledge include seeking the underlying principles in what is learned, creating models, and identifying
33:
occurs when people apply information, strategies, and skills they have learned to a new situation or context. Transfer is not a discrete activity, but is rather an integral part of the learning process. Researchers attempt to identify when and how transfer occurs and to offer strategies to improve
135:
of the skill or knowledge. Connections between past learning and new learning can provide a context or framework for the new information, helping students to determine sense and meaning, and encouraging retention of the new information. These connections can build up a framework of associative
139:
When Thorndike refers to similarity of elements between learning and transfer, the elements can be conditions or procedures. Conditions can be environmental, physical, mental, or emotional, and the possible combinations of conditions are countless. Procedures include sequences of events or
279:
Transfer is less a deliberate activity by the learner than it is a result of the environment at the time of learning. Teachers, being part of the learning environment, can be an instrument of transfer (both positive and negative). Recommendations for teaching for transfer include the
54:
which viewed the mind as a collection of separate modules or faculties assigned to various mental tasks. This approach resulted in school curricula that required students to study subjects such as mathematics and Latin in order to strengthen reasoning and memory faculties.
307:
Hugging is when the teacher encourages transfer by incorporating similarities between the learning situation and the future situations in which the learning might be used. Some methods for hugging include simulation games, mental practice, and contingency learning.
119:). However, there remains controversy as to how transfer of learning should be conceptualized and explained, what its prevalence is, what its relation is to learning in general, and whether it exists at all. 140:
information. Although the theory is that the similarity of elements facilitates transfer, there is a challenge in identifying which specific elements had an effect on the learner at the time of learning.
136:
networks that students can call upon for future problem-solving. Information stored in memory is "flexible, interpretive, generically altered, and its recall and transfer are largely context-dependent".
50:) approach to learning believed that specific mental faculties could be strengthened by particular courses of training and that these strengthened faculties transferred to other situations, based on 311:
Bridging is when the teacher encourages transfer by helping students to find connections between learning and to abstract their existing knowledge to new concepts. Some methods for bridging include
66:
in 1901 postulated that the transfer of learning was restricted or assisted by the elements in common between the original context and the next context. The notion was originally introduced as
156:
Association: connections between multiple events, actions, bits of information, and so on; as well as the conditions and emotions connected to it by the learner.
74:. Thorndike urged schools to design curricula with tasks similar to those students would encounter outside of school to facilitate the transfer of learning. 727: 252:
High-road transfer occurs when the learner consciously and deliberately ("mindfully") evaluates the new situation and applies previous learning to it.
212:
Near transfer occurs when many elements overlap between the conditions in which the learner obtained the knowledge or skill and the new situation.
691: 434: 111:
Today, transfer of learning is usually described as the process and the effective extent to which past experiences (also referred to as the
447:
Thorndike, E. L. and Woodworth, R. S. (1901) "The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions",
716: 708:
Corder, S. P. (1967). The significance of learners' errors. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 5, 161–170.
268:
High-road transfer that is backward reaching occurs when learners in a new situation think about previous situations that might apply.
288:
strategies; providing authentic environment and activities within a conceptual framework; encouraging problem-based learning;
667: 410: 169:
Transfer of learning can be cognitive, socio-emotional, or motor. The following table presents different types of transfer.
236:
Figural transfer occurs when applying general knowledge to a new situation, often making use of analogies or metaphors.
736:
Vignati R.(2009) Il transfer cognitivo nei processi di apprendimento: un paradifma del cambiamento e della creatività?
519: 749: 260:
High-road transfer that is forward reaching occurs when learners think about possible other uses while learning.
304:
Hugging and bridging as techniques for positive transfer were suggested by the research of Perkins and Salomon.
89:
The argument is also made that transfer is not distinct from learning, as people do not encounter situations as
754: 567: 514:(4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson/Merrill/Prentice Hall. pp. 20, 45, 57, 165, 217–224. 559: 733:
Tinberg, H. (2017). Teaching for Transfer: A passport for writing in new contexts. Peer Review, 19, 17–20.
590:"Transfer of Learning and Teaching: A Review of Transfer Theories and Effective Instructional Practices" 191: 150:
Similarity: commonalities between original learning and new, such as environment and other memory cues.
372: 132: 293: 220:
Far transfer occurs when the new situation is very different from that in which learning occurred.
244:
Low-road transfer occurs when well-established skills transfer spontaneously, even automatically.
161:
analogies and metaphors, all of which assist with creating associations and encouraging transfer.
481: 468: 455: 82: 362: 289: 228:
Literal transfer occurs when performing the skill exactly as learned but in a new situation.
405:. Hergenhahn, B. R., 1934-2007 (Ninth ed.). New York. pp. 37–38, 60–62, 208–209. 342: 337: 8: 63: 685: 428: 51: 24: 196:
Negative transfer occurs when prior learning hinders or interferes with new learning.
759: 673: 663: 627: 563: 525: 515: 416: 406: 352: 147:
Context and degree of original learning: how well the learner acquired the knowledge.
78: 20: 589: 485: 472: 459: 97:
considered it more a continuum, with no bright line between learning and transfer.
59: 712: 367: 357: 94: 743: 677: 631: 420: 316: 312: 105: 529: 204:
Zero transfer occurs when prior learning has no influence on new learning.
90: 728:
Rocky roads to transfer: rethinking mechanisms of a neglected phenomenon
553: 347: 489: 476: 463: 328: 186:
Positive transfer occurs when prior learning assists new learning.
108:'s concept of a response to a stimulus occurring to other stimuli. 333: 153:
Critical attributes: characteristics that make something unique.
662:(Fifth ed.). Thousand Oaks, California. pp. 154–186. 115:) affect learning and performance in a new situation (the 741: 555:Transfer: review, reconstruction, and resolution 86:will have to perform will facilitate transfer. 512:Learning theories: an educational perspective 551: 726:Salomon, G., & Perkins, D. N. (1989). 690:: CS1 maint: location missing publisher ( 433:: CS1 maint: location missing publisher ( 143:Factors that can affect transfer include: 730:. Educational Psychologist, 24, 113–142. 505: 503: 501: 499: 274: 122: 721:International Encyclopedia of Education 653: 651: 649: 647: 645: 643: 641: 583: 581: 579: 403:An introduction to theories of learning 396: 394: 392: 390: 388: 299: 742: 723:(2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press. 587: 547: 545: 543: 541: 539: 509: 164: 657: 496: 400: 638: 615: 576: 441: 385: 100:Transfer may also be referred to as 536: 13: 702: 14: 771: 621: 401:Olson, Matthew H. (2015-07-22). 658:Sousa, David A. (2016-12-02). 16:Educational psychology concept 1: 588:Jajian, Shiva (Summer 2019). 378: 58:Disputing formal discipline, 315:, developing analogies, and 7: 552:Helfenstein, Sacha (2005). 322: 296:; and game-based learning. 37: 10: 776: 597:IAFOR Journal of Education 77:In contrast to Thorndike, 18: 626:. The Macmillan Company. 373:Instructional scaffolding 624:The Transfer of Learning 510:Schunk, Dale H. (2004). 294:cognitive apprenticeship 72:identical element theory 19:Not to be confused with 560:University of Jyväskylä 750:Educational psychology 711:Perkins, D. N., & 363:Classical conditioning 755:Experiential learning 290:community of practice 275:Teaching for transfer 123:Transfer and learning 717:Transfer of learning 660:How the brain learns 449:Psychological Review 343:Priming (psychology) 338:Analogical reasoning 300:Hugging and bridging 68:transfer of practice 31:Transfer of learning 165:Transfer taxonomies 64:Robert S. Woodworth 52:faculty psychology 25:knowledge transfer 669:978-1-5063-4630-4 412:978-1-317-35068-2 353:Language transfer 272: 271: 265:Backward reaching 83:law of contiguity 79:Edwin Ray Guthrie 48:mental discipline 44:formal discipline 21:Transfer learning 767: 696: 695: 689: 681: 655: 636: 635: 619: 613: 612: 610: 608: 594: 585: 574: 573: 549: 534: 533: 507: 494: 490:10.1037/h0071363 477:10.1037/h0071280 464:10.1037/h0074898 445: 439: 438: 432: 424: 398: 257:Forward reaching 178:Characteristics 172: 171: 60:Edward Thorndike 775: 774: 770: 769: 768: 766: 765: 764: 740: 739: 705: 703:Further reading 700: 699: 683: 682: 670: 656: 639: 620: 616: 606: 604: 592: 586: 577: 570: 550: 537: 522: 508: 497: 452: 446: 442: 426: 425: 413: 399: 386: 381: 368:Gavriel Salomon 358:Problem solving 325: 302: 277: 167: 125: 117:transfer target 113:transfer source 40: 28: 17: 12: 11: 5: 773: 763: 762: 757: 752: 738: 737: 734: 731: 724: 709: 704: 701: 698: 697: 668: 637: 614: 575: 568: 535: 520: 495: 493: 492: 484:, pp. 553–564 479: 471:, pp. 384–395 466: 458:, pp. 247–261 440: 411: 383: 382: 380: 377: 376: 375: 370: 365: 360: 355: 350: 345: 340: 331: 324: 321: 301: 298: 276: 273: 270: 269: 266: 262: 261: 258: 254: 253: 250: 246: 245: 242: 238: 237: 234: 230: 229: 226: 222: 221: 218: 214: 213: 210: 206: 205: 202: 198: 197: 194: 188: 187: 184: 180: 179: 176: 166: 163: 158: 157: 154: 151: 148: 133:generalization 124: 121: 102:generalization 93:. Perkins and 39: 36: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 772: 761: 758: 756: 753: 751: 748: 747: 745: 735: 732: 729: 725: 722: 718: 714: 710: 707: 706: 693: 687: 679: 675: 671: 665: 661: 654: 652: 650: 648: 646: 644: 642: 633: 629: 625: 622:Ellis, H. C. 618: 602: 598: 591: 584: 582: 580: 571: 565: 561: 557: 556: 548: 546: 544: 542: 540: 531: 527: 523: 521:0-13-038496-8 517: 513: 506: 504: 502: 500: 491: 487: 483: 480: 478: 474: 470: 467: 465: 461: 457: 454: 453: 450: 444: 436: 430: 422: 418: 414: 408: 404: 397: 395: 393: 391: 389: 384: 374: 371: 369: 366: 364: 361: 359: 356: 354: 351: 349: 346: 344: 341: 339: 335: 332: 330: 327: 326: 320: 318: 317:metacognition 314: 313:brainstorming 309: 305: 297: 295: 291: 287: 283: 267: 264: 263: 259: 256: 255: 251: 248: 247: 243: 240: 239: 235: 232: 231: 227: 224: 223: 219: 216: 215: 211: 208: 207: 203: 200: 199: 195: 193: 190: 189: 185: 182: 181: 177: 174: 173: 170: 162: 155: 152: 149: 146: 145: 144: 141: 137: 134: 130: 127:People store 120: 118: 114: 109: 107: 106:B. F. Skinner 103: 98: 96: 92: 87: 84: 80: 75: 73: 69: 65: 61: 56: 53: 49: 45: 35: 32: 26: 22: 720: 659: 623: 617: 605:. Retrieved 600: 596: 554: 511: 448: 443: 402: 310: 306: 303: 285: 281: 278: 168: 159: 142: 138: 129:propositions 128: 126: 116: 112: 110: 101: 99: 91:blank slates 88: 76: 71: 67: 57: 47: 43: 41: 30: 29: 713:Salomon, G. 744:Categories 569:951392386X 558:(Thesis). 379:References 348:Affordance 34:transfer. 686:cite book 678:953598757 632:248523541 429:cite book 421:914472558 249:High road 760:Pedagogy 715:(1992). 530:52418092 482:Part III 329:Metaphor 323:See also 286:bridging 241:Low road 192:Negative 183:Positive 38:Overview 607:6 March 469:Part II 334:Analogy 282:hugging 233:Figural 225:Literal 95:Salomon 676:  666:  630:  566:  528:  518:  456:Part I 419:  409:  593:(PDF) 175:Type 692:link 674:OCLC 664:ISBN 628:OCLC 609:2020 564:ISBN 526:OCLC 516:ISBN 435:link 417:OCLC 407:ISBN 284:and 209:Near 201:Zero 62:and 46:(or 42:The 603:(1) 486:doi 473:doi 460:doi 451:8: 217:Far 81:'s 23:or 746:: 719:. 688:}} 684:{{ 672:. 640:^ 599:. 595:. 578:^ 562:. 538:^ 524:. 498:^ 431:}} 427:{{ 415:. 387:^ 336:, 319:. 292:; 104:, 694:) 680:. 634:. 611:. 601:7 572:. 532:. 488:: 475:: 462:: 437:) 423:. 27:.

Index

Transfer learning
knowledge transfer
faculty psychology
Edward Thorndike
Robert S. Woodworth
Edwin Ray Guthrie
law of contiguity
blank slates
Salomon
B. F. Skinner
generalization
Negative
community of practice
cognitive apprenticeship
brainstorming
metacognition
Metaphor
Analogy
Analogical reasoning
Priming (psychology)
Affordance
Language transfer
Problem solving
Classical conditioning
Gavriel Salomon
Instructional scaffolding



Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.