Knowledge

Markedness

Source đź“ť

1088:, the value of a grammar was the inverse of the number of features required in that grammar. However, Chomsky and Halle realized that their initial approach to phonological features made implausible rules and segment inventories as highly valued as natural ones. The unmarked value of a feature was cost-free with respect to the evaluation metric, while the marked feature values were counted by the metric. Segment inventories could also be evaluated according to the number of marked features. However, the use of phonological markedness as part of the evaluation metric was never able to fully account for the fact that some features are more likely than others or for the fact that phonological systems must have a certain minimal complexity and symmetry. 883:, was marked; its absence, non-nasality, was unmarked. For Jakobson and Trubetzkoy, binary phonological features formed part of a universal feature alphabet applicable to all languages. In his 1932 article "Structure of the Russian Verb", Jakobson extended the concept to grammatical meanings in which the marked element "announces the existence of A" while the unmarked element "does not announce the existence of A, i.e., does not state whether A is present or not". Forty years later, Jakobson described language by saying that "every single constituent of a linguistic system is built on an opposition of two logical contradictories: the presence of an attribute ('markedness') in contraposition to its absence ('unmarkedness')." 1062:
for studying defaults and preferences in language acquisition. What connects various approaches is a concern for the evaluation of linguistic structure, though the details of how markedness is determined and what its implications and diagnostics are varies widely. Other approaches to universal markedness relations focus on functional economic and iconic motivations, tying recurring symmetries to properties of communication channels and communication events. Croft (1990), for example, notes that asymmetries among linguistic elements may be explainable in terms economy of form, in terms of iconism between the structure of language and conceptualization of the world.
654: 969:. What is more marked in some general contexts may be less marked in other local contexts. Thus, "ant" is less marked than "ants" on the morphological level, but on the semantic (and frequency) levels it may be more marked since ants are more often encountered many at once than one at a time. Often a more general markedness relation may be reversed in a particular context. Thus, voicelessness of 1108:
constitute 'the unmarked case'; we may think of them as optimal in terms of the evaluation metric. An actual language is determined by fixing the parameters of core grammar and then adding rules or conditions, using much richer resources, ... These added properties of grammars we may think of as the syntactic analogue of irregular verbs.
918:
of grammatical items in terms of marked and unmarked features. Other semiotically-oriented work has investigated the isomorphism of form and meaning with less emphasis on invariance, including the efforts of Henning Andersen, Michael Shapiro, and Edwin Battistella. Shapiro and Andrews have especially
1061:
Markedness has been extended and reshaped over the past century and reflects a range of loosely connected theoretical approaches. From emerging in the analysis of binary oppositions, it has become a global semiotic principle, a means of encoding naturalness and language universals, and a terminology
1031:
was an influential application of markedness to typological linguistics and a break from the tradition of Jakobson and Trubetzkoy. Greenberg took frequency to be the primary determining factor of markedness in grammar and suggested that unmarked categories could be determined by "the frequency of
930:
have suggested that markedness is related to cognitive complexity—"in terms of attention, mental effort or processing time". Linguistic 'naturalists' view markedness relations in terms of the ways in which extralinguistic principles of perceptibility and psychological efficiency determine what is
1107:
We will assume that is not an 'undifferentiated' system, but rather incorporates something analogous to a 'theory of markedness' Specifically, there is a theory of core grammar with highly restricted options, limited expressive power, and a few parameters. Systems that fall within core grammar
952:
As early as the 1930s Jakobson had already suggested applying markedness to all oppositions, explicitly mentioning such pairs as life/death, liberty/bondage, sin/virtue, and holiday/working day. Linda Waugh extended this to oppositions like male/female, white/black, sighted/blind, hearing/deaf,
1116:
The distinction between core and periphery leaves us with three notions of markedness: core versus periphery, internal to the core, and internal to the periphery. The second has to do with the way parameters are set in the absence of evidence. As for the third, there are, no doubt, significant
1035:
Greenberg also applied frequency cross-linguistically, suggesting that unmarked categories would be those that are unmarked in a wide number of languages. However, critics have argued that frequency is problematic because categories that are cross-linguistically infrequent may have a high
953:
heterosexual/homosexual, right/left, fertility/barrenness, clothed/nude, and spoken language/written language. Battistella expanded this with the demonstration of how cultures align markedness values to create cohesive symbol systems, illustrating with examples based on
939:
Since a main component of markedness is the information content and information value of an element, some studies have taken markedness as an encoding of that which is unusual or informative, and this is reflected in formal probabilistic definitions of markedness and
613:(marked). Marking may be purely semantic, or may be realized as extra morphology. The term derives from the marking of a grammatical role with a suffix or another element, and has been extended to situations where there is no morphological distinction. 1117:
regularities even in departures from the core principles (for example, in irregular verb morphology in English), and it may be that peripheral constructions are related to the core in systematic ways, say by relaxing certain conditions of core grammar.
948:
known as Δp and Δp'. Conceptual familiarity with cultural norms provided by familiar categories creates a ground against which marked categories provide a figure, opening the way for markedness to be applied to cultural and social categorization.
1125:
theory, treating it as an inherent learning hierarchy which reflects the sequence in which constructions are acquired, the difficulty of acquiring certain constructions, and the transferability of rules across languages. More recently,
1015:
The common feature of the nouns that regularize the singular to match the plural is that they occur more often in pairs or groups than singly; they are said to be semantically (but not morphologically) locally unmarked in the plural.
581:
is the state of standing out as nontypical or divergent as opposed to regular or common. In a marked–unmarked relation, one term of an opposition is the broader, dominant one. The dominant default or minimum-effort form is known as
902:, Jakobson suggested a mirror-image relationship determined by a universal feature hierarchy of marked and unmarked oppositions. Today many still see Jakobson's theory of phonological acquisition as identifying useful tendencies. 1095:, markedness as feature-evaluation did not receive the same attention that it did in phonology. Chomsky came to view unmarked properties as an innate preference structure based first in constraints and later in parameters of 1043:(Δp') and Markedness (Δp) corresponding to the different directions of prediction in human association research (binary associations or distinctions) and more generally (including features with more than two distinctions). 984:
words' plural and singular forms: In West Frisian, nouns with irregular singular-plural stem variations are undergoing regularization. Usually this means that the plural is reformed to be a regular form of the singular:
879:; markedness adds the idea of hierarchy." Trubetzkoy and Jakobson analyzed phonological oppositions such as nasal versus non-nasal as defined as the presence versus the absence of nasality; the presence of the feature, 620:
more broadly, markedness is, among other things, used to distinguish two meanings of the same term, where one is common usage (unmarked sense) and the other is specialized to a certain cultural context (marked sense).
1084:. For Chomsky and Halle, phonological features went beyond a universal phonetic vocabulary to encompass an 'evaluation metric', a means of selecting the most highly valued adequate grammar. In 914:, Rodney Sangster, Yishai Tobin and others on 'semantic invariance' (different general meanings reflected in the contextual specific meanings of features) has further developed the 829:, the first term of each pair is taken as unmarked because it occurs generally in questions. For example, English speakers typically ask how old someone is; use of the marked term ( 931:
natural in language. Willi Mayerthaler, another linguist, for example, defines unmarked categories as those "in agreement with the typical attributes of the speaker".
558: 590:. In other words, markedness involves the characterization of a "normal" linguistic unit against one or more of its possible "irregular" forms. 1053:. This entails that a category is taken as marked if every language that has the marked category also has the unmarked one but not vice versa. 1490:
Eckman, F. R. (1991). "The Structural Conformity Hypothesis and the Acquisition of Consonantal Clusters in the Interlanguage of Learners".
754:, and so forth. Similarly, unaffixed masculine or singular forms are taken to be unmarked in contrast to affixed feminine or plural forms: 797:
The default nature allows unmarked lexical forms to be identified even when the opposites are not morphologically related. In the pairs
632:
is quantified as a measure of how much one variable is marked as a predictor or possible cause of another, and is also known as
1299:
Powers, David M W (2011). "Evaluation: From Precision, Recall and F-Score to ROC, Informedness, Markedness & Correlation".
1621: 551: 1039:
More recently the insights related to frequency have been formalized as chance-corrected conditional probabilities, with
701: 683: 1616: 1164: 945: 915: 544: 679: 1417:
Perruchet, P.; Peereman, R. (2004). "The exploitation of distributional information in syllable processing".
1080: 1122: 854: 675: 520: 1596: 1179: 923:
and markedness, treating it "as species of interpretant" in Peirce's sign–object–interpretant triad.
305: 255: 192: 172: 719: 664: 388: 331: 326: 144: 92: 944:
that, for dichotomous problems, correspond to the chance-correct unidirectional components of the
1050: 668: 498: 383: 336: 225: 27: 966: 503: 475: 436: 416: 371: 366: 235: 1130:
approaches emerging in the 1990s have incorporated markedness in the ranking of constraints.
981: 376: 139: 1530:
Archangeli, Diana 1997 "Optimality Theory: An Introduction to Linguistics in the 1990s", In
1000:
However, a number of words instead reform the singular by extending the form of the plural:
977:
or in the neighborhood of voiced consonants, voicing may be the expected or unmarked value.
722:
endings. Thus, a morphologically negative word form is marked as opposed to a positive one:
16:
State of standing out as unusual or difficult in comparison to a more common or regular form
1159: 891: 310: 300: 36: 8: 1046: 876: 468: 396: 60: 20: 1606: 1601: 1434: 1174: 1144: 865: 260: 240: 185: 161: 134: 1430: 1325:
Waugh, Linda "Marked and Unmarked: A Choice Between Unequals in Semiotic Structure".
1127: 1096: 1092: 872: 463: 456: 446: 411: 401: 341: 275: 215: 197: 85: 1438: 1611: 1426: 1308: 1024: 637: 406: 270: 55: 871:
Both sound and meaning were analyzed into systems of binary distinctive features.
451: 320: 282: 122: 104: 80: 75: 845:
While the idea of linguistic asymmetry predated the actual coining of the terms
714:
In terms of lexical opposites, a marked form is a non-basic one, often one with
605:
oppositions, defining them in terms of marked and unmarked oppositions, such as
954: 861: 617: 515: 346: 265: 149: 70: 1590: 1169: 1100: 895: 857: 230: 1154: 1075: 1071: 1040: 957:'s work. Other work has applied markedness to stylistics, music, and myth. 941: 927: 920: 911: 880: 480: 315: 129: 97: 65: 1366:
Musical Meaning in Beethoven: Markedness, Correlation, and Interpretation
574: 19:"Unmarked" redirects here. For undecorated law enforcement vehicles, see 1558:
Chandler, Daniel 2005 Entry on markedness. In John Protevi (ed.) (2005)
1103:
extended this to view markedness as part of a theory of 'core grammar':
1224:
Jakobson, R. (1932). "The Structure of the Russian Verb". Reprinted in
1139: 715: 625: 525: 510: 441: 250: 156: 112: 1312: 970: 786:. An unmarked form is also a default form. For example, the unmarked 602: 594: 220: 117: 653: 890:, Jakobson suggested that phonological markedness played a role in 245: 1525:
Markedness Theory: The Union of Asymmetry and Semiosis in Language
899: 598: 50: 1464:
Chomsky, Noam and Lasnik, Howard (1977). "Filters and Control".
1198:
Andersen, Henning (1989). "Markedness—The First 150 Years". In
1149: 177: 974: 1516:
Andersen, Henning 1989 "Markedness—The First 150 Years", In
960: 1099:. In their 1977 article "Filters and Control", Chomsky and 1070:
Markedness entered generative linguistic theory through
1121:
Some generative researchers have applied markedness to
934: 593:
In linguistics, markedness can apply to, among others,
875:
wrote: "Binarism suggests symmetry and equivalence in
1539:
Markedness: The Evaluative Superstructure of Language
1353:
Codes and Consequences: Choosing Linguistic Varieties
1213:
Markedness: The Evaluative Superstructure of Language
853:, the modern concept of markedness originated in the 1455:. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club. 888:
Child Language, Aphasia, and Universals of Language
1479:Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin and Use 1416: 898:. Drawing on existing studies of acquisition and 868:as a means of characterizing binary oppositions. 643: 1588: 1390:Tiersma, Peter. "Local and General Markedness", 794:is marked because it can refer only to females. 1560:Edinburgh Dictionary of Continental Philosophy 1453:The Theory of Markedness in Generative Grammar 1112:A few years later, Chomsky describes it thus: 840: 1410: 1265:Syntax: A Functional-Typological Introduction 1237:Jakobson, R. (1972). "Verbal Communication". 1226:Russian and Slavic Grammar Studies, 1931–1981 552: 26:"Marked" redirects here. For other uses, see 1562:, Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press. 1019: 682:. Unsourced material may be challenged and 1267:, vol. 2, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 1990. 1032:association of things in the real world". 636:(deltaP) in simple two-choice cases. See 559: 545: 1292: 961:Local markedness and markedness reversals 919:made connections between the semiotic of 702:Learn how and when to remove this message 1581:Key Concepts in Language and Linguistics 1301:Journal of Machine Learning Technologies 1065: 905: 1565:Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris 1968 1036:distribution in a particular language. 910:The work of Cornelius van Schooneveld, 1589: 1492:Studies in Second Language Acquisition 1298: 1518:Markedness in Synchrony and Diachrony 1215:. State University of New York Press. 1200:Markedness in Synchrony and Diachrony 992:: "koal" (coal), "kwallen" (coals) → 790:can refer to a male or female, while 1548:, New York: Oxford University Press. 1527:, Durham, NC: Duke University Press. 1254:. New York: Oxford University Press. 935:Cultural markedness and informedness 680:adding citations to reliable sources 647: 973:is typically unmarked. But between 13: 1510: 1007:: "earm" (arm), "jermen" (arms) → 996:: "Koal" (coal), "Koalen" (coals). 21:Police car § Functional types 14: 1633: 1583:, London and New York: Routledge. 980:Reversal is reflected in certain 1381:. Indiana University Press, 1989 1368:. Indiana University Press, 1994 1165:Matthews correlation coefficient 946:Matthews correlation coefficient 652: 628:, the social science concept of 186:Singulative-Collective-Plurative 1497: 1484: 1471: 1458: 1445: 1397: 1384: 1371: 1358: 1345: 1332: 1319: 1011:: "jerm" (arm), "jermen" (arms) 1532:Optimality Theory: An Overview 1289:Battistella, Markedness, 1990. 1283: 1270: 1257: 1244: 1231: 1218: 1205: 1192: 1056: 644:Marked and unmarked word pairs 586:; the other, secondary one is 150:Suffixaufnahme (case stacking) 1: 1431:10.1016/s0911-6044(03)00059-9 1185: 926:Functional linguists such as 1567:The Sound Pattern of English 1520:. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 1202:. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 1086:The Sound Pattern of English 1081:The Sound Pattern of English 1049:have also been connected to 7: 1622:Social sciences terminology 1569:, New York: Harper and Row. 1551:Chandler, Daniel 2002/2007 1351:Myers-Scotton, Carol (ed.) 1250:Battistella, Edwin (1996). 1211:Battistella, Edwin (1990). 1133: 1123:second-language acquisition 841:Background in Prague School 266:Lexical aspect (Aktionsart) 10: 1638: 1576:, The Hague: Mouton, 1966. 1451:Kean, Mary-Louise (1980). 1280:. Ann Arbor: Karoma, 1988. 837:) would presuppose youth. 25: 18: 1541:, Albany, NY: SUNY Press. 1278:Morphological Naturalness 1180:Underlying representation 1544:Battistella, Edwin 1996 1537:Battistella, Edwin 1990 1534:. Malden, MA: Blackwell. 1020:Universals and frequency 389:Serial verb construction 1617:Linguistics terminology 1546:The Logic of Markedness 1405:The Logic of Markedness 1252:The Logic of Markedness 327:Honorifics (politeness) 28:Marked (disambiguation) 1477:Chomsky, Noam (1986). 1119: 1110: 965:Markedness depends on 504:Polypersonal agreement 1553:Semiotics: The Basics 1114: 1105: 1066:In generative grammar 906:Jakobsonian tradition 577:and social sciences, 140:Genitive construction 1555:, London: Routledge. 1379:The Semiotic of Myth 1342:, 1990, pp. 188–189. 1160:Marker (linguistics) 892:language acquisition 676:improve this section 393:Traditional grammar 361:Syntax relationships 37:Grammatical features 1574:Language Universals 1523:Andrews, Edna 1990 1419:J. Neurolinguistics 1403:Battistella, Edwin 1338:Battistella, Edwin 1329:; 38: 299–318, 1982 1239:Scientific American 1029:Language Universals 877:linguistic analysis 311:Comparison (degree) 61:Dative construction 1579:Trask, R. L. 1999 1572:Greenberg, Joseph 1466:Linguistic Inquiry 1276:Mayerthaler Willi 1175:Other (philosophy) 1145:Lemma (morphology) 1051:implicational laws 866:Nikolai Trubetzkoy 640:for more details. 261:Grammatical aspect 1377:Liszka, James J. 1128:optimality theory 1097:universal grammar 1093:generative syntax 1009:regularized forms 994:regularized forms 916:semantic analysis 873:Edwin Battistella 712: 711: 704: 569: 568: 464:Topic and Comment 447:Thematic relation 342:Reflexive pronoun 256:Tense–aspect–mood 216:Associated motion 198:Universal grinder 1629: 1597:Sociolinguistics 1504: 1503:Archangeli 1997. 1501: 1495: 1488: 1482: 1475: 1469: 1462: 1456: 1449: 1443: 1442: 1414: 1408: 1401: 1395: 1388: 1382: 1375: 1369: 1362: 1356: 1349: 1343: 1336: 1330: 1323: 1317: 1316: 1296: 1290: 1287: 1281: 1274: 1268: 1261: 1255: 1248: 1242: 1235: 1229: 1222: 1216: 1209: 1203: 1196: 1025:Joseph Greenberg 707: 700: 696: 693: 687: 656: 648: 638:confusion matrix 635: 561: 554: 547: 295:General features 210:Related to verbs 45:Related to nouns 33: 32: 1637: 1636: 1632: 1631: 1630: 1628: 1627: 1626: 1587: 1586: 1513: 1511:Further reading 1508: 1507: 1502: 1498: 1489: 1485: 1476: 1472: 1463: 1459: 1450: 1446: 1425:(2–3): 97–119. 1415: 1411: 1402: 1398: 1389: 1385: 1376: 1372: 1364:Hatten, Robert 1363: 1359: 1350: 1346: 1337: 1333: 1324: 1320: 1297: 1293: 1288: 1284: 1275: 1271: 1262: 1258: 1249: 1245: 1236: 1232: 1228:. Mouton, 1984. 1223: 1219: 1210: 1206: 1197: 1193: 1188: 1136: 1068: 1059: 1022: 963: 937: 908: 843: 708: 697: 691: 688: 673: 657: 646: 633: 618:social sciences 609:(unmarked) vs. 565: 536: 535: 494: 486: 485: 432: 424: 423: 362: 354: 353: 323:(verbal number) 321:Pluractionality 296: 288: 287: 211: 203: 202: 182: 123:Collective noun 105:Construct state 46: 31: 24: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1635: 1625: 1624: 1619: 1614: 1609: 1604: 1599: 1585: 1584: 1577: 1570: 1563: 1556: 1549: 1542: 1535: 1528: 1521: 1512: 1509: 1506: 1505: 1496: 1483: 1470: 1457: 1444: 1409: 1407:, 1996, p. 51. 1396: 1383: 1370: 1357: 1355:. Oxford, 1998 1344: 1331: 1318: 1291: 1282: 1269: 1256: 1243: 1230: 1217: 1204: 1190: 1189: 1187: 1184: 1183: 1182: 1177: 1172: 1167: 1162: 1157: 1152: 1147: 1142: 1135: 1132: 1067: 1064: 1058: 1055: 1021: 1018: 1013: 1012: 998: 997: 962: 959: 955:Rodney Needham 936: 933: 907: 904: 862:Roman Jakobson 842: 839: 710: 709: 660: 658: 651: 645: 642: 567: 566: 564: 563: 556: 549: 541: 538: 537: 534: 533: 528: 523: 518: 516:Empty category 513: 508: 507: 506: 495: 492: 491: 488: 487: 484: 483: 478: 473: 472: 471: 461: 460: 459: 454: 444: 439: 433: 430: 429: 426: 425: 422: 421: 420: 419: 414: 409: 404: 399: 391: 386: 381: 380: 379: 374: 363: 360: 359: 356: 355: 352: 351: 350: 349: 347:Reflexive verb 344: 334: 329: 324: 318: 313: 308: 303: 297: 294: 293: 290: 289: 286: 285: 280: 279: 278: 273: 268: 263: 253: 248: 243: 238: 233: 228: 223: 218: 212: 209: 208: 205: 204: 201: 200: 195: 190: 189: 188: 183: 181: 180: 175: 170: 166: 159: 154: 153: 152: 147: 137: 132: 127: 126: 125: 120: 115: 107: 102: 101: 100: 90: 89: 88: 83: 78: 73: 71:Quirky subject 68: 63: 53: 47: 44: 43: 40: 39: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1634: 1623: 1620: 1618: 1615: 1613: 1610: 1608: 1605: 1603: 1600: 1598: 1595: 1594: 1592: 1582: 1578: 1575: 1571: 1568: 1564: 1561: 1557: 1554: 1550: 1547: 1543: 1540: 1536: 1533: 1529: 1526: 1522: 1519: 1515: 1514: 1500: 1494:13(1), 23–41. 1493: 1487: 1480: 1474: 1468:8.3: 425–504. 1467: 1461: 1454: 1448: 1440: 1436: 1432: 1428: 1424: 1420: 1413: 1406: 1400: 1393: 1387: 1380: 1374: 1367: 1361: 1354: 1348: 1341: 1335: 1328: 1322: 1314: 1310: 1306: 1302: 1295: 1286: 1279: 1273: 1266: 1260: 1253: 1247: 1240: 1234: 1227: 1221: 1214: 1208: 1201: 1195: 1191: 1181: 1178: 1176: 1173: 1171: 1170:Null morpheme 1168: 1166: 1163: 1161: 1158: 1156: 1153: 1151: 1148: 1146: 1143: 1141: 1138: 1137: 1131: 1129: 1124: 1118: 1113: 1109: 1104: 1102: 1101:Howard Lasnik 1098: 1094: 1089: 1087: 1083: 1082: 1077: 1073: 1063: 1054: 1052: 1048: 1044: 1042: 1037: 1033: 1030: 1027:'s 1966 book 1026: 1017: 1010: 1006: 1003: 1002: 1001: 995: 991: 988: 987: 986: 983: 978: 976: 972: 968: 958: 956: 950: 947: 943: 932: 929: 924: 922: 917: 913: 903: 901: 897: 893: 889: 884: 882: 878: 874: 869: 867: 863: 859: 858:structuralism 856: 855:Prague School 852: 848: 838: 836: 834: 828: 824: 820: 816: 812: 808: 804: 800: 795: 793: 789: 785: 781: 777: 773: 769: 765: 761: 757: 753: 749: 745: 741: 737: 733: 729: 725: 721: 717: 706: 703: 695: 692:December 2019 685: 681: 677: 671: 670: 666: 661:This section 659: 655: 650: 649: 641: 639: 631: 627: 622: 619: 614: 612: 608: 604: 600: 596: 591: 589: 585: 580: 576: 571: 562: 557: 555: 550: 548: 543: 542: 540: 539: 532: 529: 527: 524: 522: 521:Incorporation 519: 517: 514: 512: 509: 505: 502: 501: 500: 497: 496: 490: 489: 482: 479: 477: 474: 470: 467: 466: 465: 462: 458: 455: 453: 450: 449: 448: 445: 443: 440: 438: 435: 434: 428: 427: 418: 415: 413: 410: 408: 405: 403: 400: 398: 395: 394: 392: 390: 387: 385: 382: 378: 375: 373: 370: 369: 368: 365: 364: 358: 357: 348: 345: 343: 340: 339: 338: 335: 333: 330: 328: 325: 322: 319: 317: 314: 312: 309: 307: 304: 302: 299: 298: 292: 291: 284: 281: 277: 274: 272: 269: 267: 264: 262: 259: 258: 257: 254: 252: 249: 247: 244: 242: 239: 237: 234: 232: 231:Evidentiality 229: 227: 224: 222: 219: 217: 214: 213: 207: 206: 199: 196: 194: 191: 187: 184: 179: 176: 174: 171: 168: 167: 165: 164: 163: 160: 158: 155: 151: 148: 146: 143: 142: 141: 138: 136: 133: 131: 128: 124: 121: 119: 116: 114: 111: 110: 109:Countability 108: 106: 103: 99: 96: 95: 94: 91: 87: 84: 82: 79: 77: 74: 72: 69: 67: 64: 62: 59: 58: 57: 54: 52: 49: 48: 42: 41: 38: 35: 34: 29: 22: 1580: 1573: 1566: 1559: 1552: 1545: 1538: 1531: 1524: 1517: 1499: 1491: 1486: 1478: 1473: 1465: 1460: 1452: 1447: 1422: 1418: 1412: 1404: 1399: 1391: 1386: 1378: 1373: 1365: 1360: 1352: 1347: 1339: 1334: 1326: 1321: 1307:(1): 37–63. 1304: 1300: 1294: 1285: 1277: 1272: 1264: 1259: 1251: 1246: 1238: 1233: 1225: 1220: 1212: 1207: 1199: 1194: 1155:Male as norm 1120: 1115: 1111: 1106: 1090: 1085: 1079: 1076:Morris Halle 1072:Noam Chomsky 1069: 1060: 1045: 1041:Informedness 1038: 1034: 1028: 1023: 1014: 1008: 1005:Old paradigm 1004: 999: 993: 990:Old paradigm 989: 982:West Frisian 979: 964: 951: 942:informedness 938: 925: 921:C. S. Peirce 912:Edna Andrews 909: 887: 886:In his 1941 885: 870: 850: 846: 844: 832: 830: 826: 822: 818: 814: 810: 806: 802: 798: 796: 791: 787: 783: 779: 775: 771: 767: 763: 759: 755: 751: 747: 743: 739: 735: 731: 727: 723: 720:derivational 716:inflectional 713: 698: 689: 674:Please help 662: 629: 623: 615: 610: 606: 595:phonological 592: 587: 583: 578: 572: 570: 530: 481:Veridicality 372:Transitivity 316:Egophoricity 130:Definiteness 98:Measure word 86:Instrumental 66:Dative shift 1241:227: 72–80. 1057:Diagnostics 928:Talmy GivĂłn 776:automobiles 599:grammatical 575:linguistics 417:Predicative 337:Reciprocity 306:Boundedness 226:Conjugation 193:Specificity 1591:Categories 1481:. Praeger. 1340:Markedness 1313:2328/27165 1263:GivĂłn, T. 1186:References 1140:Inflection 1047:Universals 971:consonants 772:automobile 630:markedness 626:psychology 579:markedness 531:Markedness 526:Inflection 511:Declension 442:Mirativity 251:Mirativity 157:Noun class 145:Possession 113:Count noun 93:Classifier 81:Comitative 76:Nominative 1607:Semantics 1602:Phonology 1327:Semiotica 736:dishonest 663:does not 611:dishonest 499:Agreement 493:Phenomena 431:Semantics 397:Predicate 384:Branching 221:Clusivity 118:Mass noun 1439:17104364 1392:Language 1134:See also 881:nasality 851:unmarked 835:are you? 784:children 603:semantic 584:unmarked 476:Volition 437:Contrast 367:Argument 332:Polarity 246:Telicity 236:Modality 169:Singular 1612:Grammar 1394:, 1982. 967:context 900:aphasia 792:lioness 768:hostess 760:lioness 752:unclean 728:unhappy 684:removed 669:sources 457:Patient 412:Adjunct 402:Subject 377:Valency 51:Animacy 1437:  1150:Lexeme 975:vowels 847:marked 811:little 744:unfair 732:honest 607:honest 601:, and 588:marked 407:Object 301:Affect 241:Person 178:Plural 162:Number 135:Gender 1435:S2CID 833:young 827:dirty 823:clean 815:happy 803:young 780:child 748:clean 724:happy 469:Focus 452:Agent 283:Voice 276:Tense 1074:and 896:loss 894:and 864:and 849:and 831:how 788:lion 764:host 756:lion 740:fair 667:any 665:cite 271:Mood 173:Dual 56:Case 1427:doi 1309:hdl 1091:In 1078:'s 860:of 819:sad 807:big 799:old 718:or 678:by 624:In 616:In 573:In 1593:: 1433:. 1423:17 1421:. 1303:. 821:, 813:, 805:, 778:, 770:, 762:, 746:, 738:, 730:, 634:Δp 597:, 1441:. 1429:: 1315:. 1311:: 1305:2 825:/ 817:/ 809:/ 801:/ 782:/ 774:/ 766:/ 758:/ 750:/ 742:/ 734:/ 726:/ 705:) 699:( 694:) 690:( 686:. 672:. 560:e 553:t 546:v 30:. 23:.

Index

Police car § Functional types
Marked (disambiguation)
Grammatical features
Animacy
Case
Dative construction
Dative shift
Quirky subject
Nominative
Comitative
Instrumental
Classifier
Measure word
Construct state
Count noun
Mass noun
Collective noun
Definiteness
Gender
Genitive construction
Possession
Suffixaufnahme (case stacking)
Noun class
Number
Dual
Plural
Singulative-Collective-Plurative
Specificity
Universal grinder
Associated motion

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑