Knowledge

Ideal free distribution

Source đź“ť

202:
upheld as long as these differences are accounted for. In accounting for this variety of competitive weights, animals distribute such that their competitive weights in each habitat match the proportion of resources present there. For example, in one experiment goldfish differing in competitive ability behaved in a way that maximized their intake rate relative to their competitive weight. Since the mean rank of fish in a site varied inversely with the total number of fish in both the high resource density site and the low resource density site, there was no correlation between competitive ability and time spent at the higher resource density site. As expected in an ideally distributed population of goldfish of different competitive abilities, the intake rate of each competitive weight did not differ between the sites.
255:
involved groups of participants choosing between blue and red cards in order to earn points towards prizes. When the groups’ choice of cards was graphed in relation to the ratios between the points, the slopes demonstrated some undermatching, which is a deviation from the Matching Law. Undermatching is the situation when the ratio of foragers between two patches (in this case, how many people picked each card) is less than the ratio of resources between the two patches (the points each card is worth). The results show that the IFD could not predict the outcome. However, they also show that it is possible to apply the Ideal Free Distribution to group choice, if that group choice is motivated by the individuals’ tendencies to maximize
66:" implies that animals are aware of each patch's quality, and they choose to forage in the patch with the highest quality. The term "free" implies that animals are capable of moving unhindered from one patch to another. Although these assumptions are not always upheld in nature, there are still many experiments that have been performed in support of IFD, even if populations naturally deviate between patches before reaching IFD. IFD theory can still be used to analyze foraging behaviors of animals, whether those behaviors support IFD, or violate it. 189:, a species of social spiders, live together cooperatively and build large web communities. Number of insects caught decreases with increasing population due to surface area scaling, but prey mass increased due to larger webs. At intermediate population size of 1000, prey biomass per capita was maximized. The results correspond to observed results of population size and ecological conditions- areas that lack larger insects have smaller spider communities. 156: 292:
account for under-matching when the distribution is less extreme than the resource rate. When one patch is seen to have more preference over another, bias in the resource ratio is taken into consideration. These two matching relationships are assessed by a regression of the log ratio of the numbers at each site against the log ratio of resources at the site.
206:
from the distribution of the competitive weights. When exposed to a poor patch and a good patch, the fish distributed such that the payoffs per unit of competitive weight were the same at both patches. This experiment demonstrates that the incorporation of competitive weights into habitat selection can improve predictions of animal distributions.
146:. Various deviations may occur initially, but eventually the patches will accommodate the number of individuals that is proportional to the amount of resources they each contain. In this case, patches of equal intrinsic value allow for the same number of individuals in each patch. At this point, the state of the individuals is referred to as 220:
reproductive success, but the aphids settle in such a way that the average reproductive success for individuals on leaves with one, two, or three galls is the same. However, reproductive success is unequal within the same leaf, and stem mothers that settle closer to the base of the leaf have higher fitness than those that settle distally.
296:
less on the more resource abundant site. Knowledge of the competitive interactions, effects of travel between sites, number of animals in population, perceptual abilities of these animals, and the relative and absolute resource availability on each patch is required to accurately predict the distribution of a foraging population.
295:
The results they found do not support IFD predictions and some take this outcome to mean that the current model is too simple. Animal behaviorists have proposed a modification to the model that denotes an ultimate outcome of a population always having more individuals on the least profitable site and
271:
In experiments that test the predictions of IFD, most often there tends to be more individuals in the least profitable patch and a shortage at the richest patch. This distribution is found across species of insects, fish and birds. However, modifications to the original assumptions have been made and
227:
distributions that each maintains ideal free distribution. For example, if good competitors forage twice as well as poor competitors, a possible scenario upholding IFD would be for four good competitors and eight poor competitors to forage at a given site, each gaining the same net payoff per unit of
192:
Bumblebees distribute themselves systematically so that there was an equalization of gain per flower (the currency) in flowers of different nectar production. Bees were also distributed proportionally based on plant density and differential nectar distribution. In Selous wild dogs, observed pack size
205:
Additionally, foraging behavior in coho salmon does not uphold ideal free distribution predicted by the equal competitors model, but does uphold ideal free distribution with the inclusion of competitive inequalities. In other words, the distribution of the number of fish was significantly different
267:
It is important to keep in mind that IFD does rely on the assumptions previously stated and that all of these qualities are probably not met in the wild. Some believe that tests of IFD are not executed properly and therefore yield results that appear to follow the prediction but in reality do not.
201:
The ideal free distribution hypothesis assumes that all individuals are equal in competitive abilities. However, there is experimental evidence that demonstrates that even when the competitive abilities, or weights, of individuals in a population differ, the ideal free distribution is still mostly
168:
despite the fact that there is an unequal number of competitors in each patch. This equilibrium is demonstrated as the red line in Figure 1, where the feeding rate is the same for all individuals even though there are 5 individuals in Patch A and 8 individuals in Patch B. From the figure, we can
254:
However, this prediction assumes that each individual will act on its own. It does not hold for situations involving group choice, which is an example of social behavior. In 2001, Kraft et al. performed an experiment that tested the IFD's predictions of group choice using humans. This experiment
291:
in order to reanalyze IFD experiments. When psychologists perform tests of this law, they use more sensitive measures to account for deviation from strict matching relationships. Kennedy and Gray utilize this method to test the validity of previous IFD experiments. Using this analysis, they can
219:
has also been shown to generally follow the Ideal Free Distribution. After hatching in the spring, female aphids compete with each other for galling sites closest to the stems of the largest leaves. Both settling on a smaller leaf and sharing a leaf with another aphid reduce a stem mother's
163:
For patches with unequal innate values, or intrinsic values, we can still apply the same distribution principle. However, it is predicted that the number of individuals in each patch will differ, as the amount of resources in each patch will be unequal. They will still reach
250:
of individual choice, which states that an individual's rate of response will be proportional to the positive reinforcement that individual receives for that response. So an animal will go to the patch that provides the most benefits to them.
169:
infer that the first 6 foragers settle in Patch B due to its greater intrinsic quality, but the increased competition causes the lesser quality Patch A to be more beneficial for the seventh individual. This figure is depicting the
89:
that is determined by the amount of resources available in each patch. Given that there is not yet any competition in each patch, individuals can assess the quality of each patch based merely on the resources available.
236:
Though the Ideal Free Distribution can be used to explain the behaviors of several species, it is not a perfect model. There remain many situations in which the IFD does not accurately predict the behavioral outcome.
527:
Kennedy, M., & Gray, R. D. (1993). Can ecological theory predict the distribution of foraging animals? a critical analysis of experiments on the ideal free distribution. Oikos, 68(1), 158-166. Retrieved from
245:
As an optimal foraging model, the Ideal Free Distribution predicts that the ratio of individuals between two foraging sites will match the ratio of resources in those two sites. This prediction is similar to the
275:
One experiment displayed this violation of IFD in stickleback fish. He saw that the actual observations and the ones stated by IFD were not congruent. More fish tended to disperse in the patch with less
228:
competitive weight. Additional combinations upholding IFD could exist as well. Even when individuals move between patches in a suboptimal fashion, this distribution of possible equilibria is unaffected.
549:
Godin, J.-G. & Keenleyside, M.H.A. 1984. Foraging on patchily distributed prey by a chichlid fish (Teleostei Cichlidae): a test of the ideal free distribution theory. Animal Behaviour 32: 120-131.
508:
Kraft, J. R., Baum, W. M., & Burge, M. J. (2002). Group choice and individual choices: modeling human social behavior with the ideal free distribution. Behavioural Processes, 57(2-3), 227-240.
100:
to move to the highest quality patch. However, this can be violated by dominant individuals within a species who may keep a weaker individual from reaching the ideal patch.
438:
Sutherland, W.J., C.R. Townsend, and J.M. Patmore. "A test of the ideal free distribution with unequal competitors." Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 23.1 (1988): 51-53.
193:
did not agree with results of daily per capita food intake. However, when factoring in distance traveled to hunt into the currency, observed pack size was close to optimal.
376:
Yip, E., Powers, K., & Aviles, L. (2008). Cooperative capture of large prey solves scaling challenge faced by spider societies. PNAS, 105(33), 11818-11822.
354:
Fretwell, S. D. & Lucas, H. L., Jr. 1969. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. I. Theoretical Development.
152:. Once the individuals are in Nash equilibrium, any migration to a different patch will be disadvantageous since all individuals obtain the same benefits. 55:
to the amount of resources available in each. For example, if patch A contains twice as many resources as patch B, there will be twice as many individuals
447:
Grand, Tamara. "Foraging site selection by juvenile coho salmon: ideal free distributions of unequal competitors." Animal Behavior. 53.1 (1997): 185-196.
499:
Ruxton, Graeme, and Stuart Humphries. "Multiple ideal free distributions of unequal competitors." Evolutionary Ecology Research. 1.5 (1999): 635-640.
409:
Abraham, J.N. (2005). Insect choice and floral size dimorphism: Sexual selection or natural selection? Journal of Insect Behavior, 18(6), 743–756.
1340: 593: 142:
Once the assumptions are met, IFD theory predicts that a population of individuals will distribute themselves equally among patches with the
561: 1484: 1554: 1126: 1091: 421:
Creel & Creel. (1995). Communal hunting and pack size in African wild dogs, Lycaon pictus. Animal Behaviour, 50(5), 1325-1339.
1564: 1292: 1569: 223:
Variations in competitive abilities of individuals in a given population also tend to result in several different possible
173:, through which the ratio of individuals at the patches corresponds to the ratio of resources available in those patches. 1757: 1510: 1333: 1174: 586: 540:
Milinski, M. 1979. An evolutionarily stable feeding strategy in sticklebacks. Zietschrit fur Tierpsychologie 51: 36-40.
846: 63: 567:
Population Ecology of Individuals chapter on Whitham's 1980 study regarding aphids and the ideal free distribution.
1307: 566: 457:
Whitham, Thomas G. (April 1980). "The Theory of Habitat Selection: Examined and Extended Using Pemphigus Aphids".
116:
Increasing the number of individuals in a given patch reduces the quality of that patch, through either increased
1589: 1302: 1169: 881: 1974: 1847: 367:
Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L.-A., & CĂ©zilly, F. (2008). Behavioural ecology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
284:
also displayed the same subtle difference in predicted vs. actual dispersal numbers in relation to resources.
2020: 1619: 1574: 1326: 579: 52: 1106: 1452: 2145: 1809: 700: 1874: 1594: 1081: 798: 695: 74:
The ideal free distribution theory is based on several assumptions and predictions as indicated below;
2055: 1667: 1559: 1417: 1402: 1397: 1076: 788: 571: 268:
This causes animal behaviorists to be split in opinions of whether IFD is a true phenomenon or not.
51:. The theory states that the number of individual animals that will aggregate in various patches is 2045: 2040: 2010: 1814: 1277: 1159: 121: 1318: 949: 1889: 1662: 1530: 1412: 1382: 1239: 1204: 924: 891: 866: 315: 2035: 1979: 1914: 1777: 1712: 1647: 1209: 997: 705: 685: 310: 256: 1939: 1884: 1747: 1732: 1515: 1472: 1462: 1457: 1214: 1194: 1050: 1040: 982: 977: 813: 665: 44: 2065: 2030: 2025: 1949: 1944: 1899: 1797: 1767: 1762: 1614: 1477: 1467: 1012: 851: 640: 215: 210: 117: 8: 2115: 2090: 1954: 1924: 1869: 1782: 1672: 1657: 1604: 1437: 1372: 1254: 1184: 1116: 715: 305: 2126: 2075: 2070: 1879: 1842: 1584: 1540: 1505: 1362: 1287: 1189: 1121: 1111: 1045: 992: 803: 748: 710: 635: 482: 474: 280:(the sought after food source) and the more abundant patch had a shortage of visitors. 36: 513: 2015: 1984: 1772: 1599: 1407: 1272: 1249: 987: 763: 675: 660: 645: 625: 426: 321: 48: 486: 1969: 1832: 1824: 1742: 1624: 1609: 1545: 1525: 1442: 1432: 1427: 1392: 1224: 1164: 1035: 836: 778: 690: 650: 509: 466: 422: 377: 165: 148: 40: 2105: 1964: 1934: 1929: 1919: 1852: 1837: 1717: 1697: 1579: 1447: 1353: 1244: 1154: 1096: 680: 606: 394: 2085: 1909: 1862: 1792: 1787: 1682: 1549: 1422: 1229: 1219: 1199: 1002: 967: 906: 783: 738: 630: 562:
The ideal free distribution when the resource is variable – Introduction
2139: 2110: 1086: 1060: 1017: 1007: 962: 929: 821: 655: 610: 106:
Individuals are aware of the value of each patch so that they can choose the
410: 393:
Dreisig, H. (1995). Ideal free distributions of nectar foraging bumblebees.
381: 2095: 2080: 1737: 1707: 1652: 1535: 1500: 1377: 876: 288: 281: 247: 1387: 934: 896: 871: 861: 826: 773: 753: 155: 2100: 1677: 1642: 1282: 1234: 1179: 1149: 1055: 972: 916: 793: 743: 529: 478: 398: 29: 2005: 1959: 1687: 1131: 1101: 901: 856: 831: 768: 758: 733: 725: 670: 601: 134:, so they are all equally able to forage and choose the ideal patch. 2060: 1989: 1520: 1348: 1027: 939: 886: 841: 470: 56: 2050: 1857: 1727: 1722: 1349: 1297: 957: 602: 277: 17: 35:
s individuals distribute themselves among several patches of
262: 325:(organism, bee, that exhibits Ideal Free Distribution) 2137: 213:stem mothers for galling sites on the leaves of 272:are implemented in experiments involving IFD. 69: 1334: 587: 345:. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 450: 1555:Latitudinal gradients in species diversity 1341: 1327: 594: 580: 411:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10905-005-8737-1 240: 1453:Predator–prey (Lotka–Volterra) equations 1092:Tritrophic interactions in plant defense 209:In another example, competition between 154: 1485:Random generalized Lotka–Volterra model 523: 521: 456: 263:Experimental data not in support of IFD 2138: 1293:Herbivore adaptations to plant defense 196: 1322: 575: 502: 343:Populations in a Seasonal Environment 1308:Predator avoidance in schooling fish 530:https://www.jstor.org/stable/3545322 518: 441: 415: 403: 399:https://www.jstor.org/stable/3546218 387: 348: 181: 1758:Intermediate disturbance hypothesis 543: 534: 493: 432: 361: 335: 13: 1511:Ecological effects of biodiversity 370: 28:) is a theoretical way in which a 14: 2157: 847:Generalist and specialist species 555: 1570:Occupancy–abundance relationship 1590:Relative abundance distribution 1303:Plant defense against herbivory 1170:Competitive exclusion principle 882:Mesopredator release hypothesis 231: 1175:Consumer–resource interactions 287:Kennedy and Gray utilized the 137: 77: 1: 2021:Biological data visualization 1848:Environmental niche modelling 1575:Population viability analysis 514:10.1016/S0376-6357(02)00016-5 329: 1506:Density-dependent inhibition 427:10.1016/0003-3472(95)80048-4 85:Each available patch has an 7: 1975:Liebig's law of the minimum 1810:Resource selection function 701:Metabolic theory of ecology 299: 70:Assumptions and predictions 10: 2162: 1875:Niche apportionment models 1595:Relative species abundance 799:Primary nutritional groups 696:List of feeding behaviours 176: 59:in patch A as in patch B. 2124: 2056:Ecosystem based fisheries 1998: 1898: 1823: 1696: 1668:Interspecific competition 1633: 1560:Minimum viable population 1493: 1418:Maximum sustainable yield 1403:Intraspecific competition 1398:Effective population size 1361: 1278:Anti-predator adaptations 1263: 1142: 1069: 1026: 948: 915: 812: 789:Photosynthetic efficiency 724: 618: 2046:Ecological stoichiometry 2011:Alternative stable state 122:interference competition 1890:Ontogenetic niche shift 1753:Ideal free distribution 1663:Ecological facilitation 1413:Malthusian growth model 1383:Consumer-resource model 1240:Paradox of the plankton 1205:Energy systems language 925:Chemoorganoheterotrophy 892:Optimal foraging theory 867:Heterotrophic nutrition 459:The American Naturalist 382:10.1073/pnas.0710603105 316:Optimal foraging theory 171:habitat matching effect 43:, in order to minimize 22:ideal free distribution 2036:Ecological forecasting 1980:Marginal value theorem 1778:Landscape epidemiology 1713:Cross-boundary subsidy 1648:Biological interaction 998:Microbial intelligence 686:Green world hypothesis 341:Fretwell, S. D. 1972. 311:Marginal value theorem 257:positive reinforcement 241:Deviation from the IFD 160: 2041:Ecological humanities 1940:Ecological energetics 1885:Niche differentiation 1748:Habitat fragmentation 1516:Ecological extinction 1463:Small population size 1215:Feed conversion ratio 1195:Ecological succession 1127:San Francisco Estuary 1041:Ecological efficiency 983:Microbial cooperation 158: 2066:Evolutionary ecology 2031:Ecological footprint 2026:Ecological economics 1950:Ecological threshold 1945:Ecological indicator 1815:Source–sink dynamics 1768:Land change modeling 1763:Insular biogeography 1615:Species distribution 1354:Modelling ecosystems 1013:Microbial metabolism 852:Intraguild predation 641:Biogeochemical cycle 607:Modelling ecosystems 216:Populus angustifolia 211:sugarbeet root aphid 144:same intrinsic value 130:All individuals are 118:scramble competition 45:resource competition 2116:Theoretical ecology 2091:Natural environment 1955:Ecosystem diversity 1925:Ecological collapse 1915:Bateman's principle 1870:Limiting similarity 1783:Landscape limnology 1605:Species homogeneity 1443:Population modeling 1438:Population dynamics 1255:Trophic state index 306:Behavioural ecology 197:Unequal competitors 132:competitively equal 2146:Population ecology 2127:Outline of ecology 2076:Industrial ecology 2071:Functional ecology 1935:Ecological deficit 1880:Niche construction 1843:Ecosystem engineer 1620:Species–area curve 1541:Introduced species 1356:: Other components 1288:Deimatic behaviour 1190:Ecological network 1122:North Pacific Gyre 1107:hydrothermal vents 1046:Ecological pyramid 993:Microbial food web 804:Primary production 749:Foundation species 397:, 72(2), 161-172. 356:Acta Biotheoretica 187:Anelosimus eximius 161: 87:individual quality 2133: 2132: 2016:Balance of nature 1773:Landscape ecology 1658:Community ecology 1600:Species diversity 1536:Indicator species 1531:Gradient analysis 1408:Logistic function 1316: 1315: 1273:Animal coloration 1250:Trophic mutualism 988:Microbial ecology 779:Photoheterotrophs 764:Myco-heterotrophy 676:Ecosystem ecology 661:Carrying capacity 626:Abiotic component 322:Xylocopa sonorina 182:Experimental data 2153: 1833:Ecological niche 1805:selection theory 1625:Umbrella species 1610:Species richness 1546:Invasive species 1526:Flagship species 1433:Population cycle 1428:Overexploitation 1393:Ecological yield 1343: 1336: 1329: 1320: 1319: 1225:Mesotrophic soil 1165:Climax community 1097:Marine food webs 1036:Biomagnification 837:Chemoorganotroph 691:Keystone species 651:Biotic component 596: 589: 582: 573: 572: 550: 547: 541: 538: 532: 525: 516: 506: 500: 497: 491: 490: 454: 448: 445: 439: 436: 430: 419: 413: 407: 401: 391: 385: 374: 368: 365: 359: 358:19: 16–36. 352: 346: 339: 225:Nash equilibrium 166:Nash equilibrium 149:Nash equilibrium 96:Individuals are 34: 2161: 2160: 2156: 2155: 2154: 2152: 2151: 2150: 2136: 2135: 2134: 2129: 2120: 2106:Systems ecology 1994: 1965:Extinction debt 1930:Ecological debt 1920:Bioluminescence 1901: 1894: 1863:marine habitats 1838:Ecological trap 1819: 1699: 1692: 1635: 1629: 1585:Rapoport's rule 1580:Priority effect 1521:Endemic species 1489: 1448:Population size 1364: 1357: 1347: 1317: 1312: 1265: 1259: 1245:Trophic cascade 1155:Bioaccumulation 1138: 1065: 1022: 944: 911: 808: 720: 681:Ecosystem model 614: 600: 558: 553: 548: 544: 539: 535: 526: 519: 507: 503: 498: 494: 455: 451: 446: 442: 437: 433: 420: 416: 408: 404: 392: 388: 375: 371: 366: 362: 353: 349: 340: 336: 332: 302: 265: 243: 234: 199: 184: 179: 140: 80: 72: 32: 12: 11: 5: 2159: 2149: 2148: 2131: 2130: 2125: 2122: 2121: 2119: 2118: 2113: 2108: 2103: 2098: 2093: 2088: 2086:Microecosystem 2083: 2078: 2073: 2068: 2063: 2058: 2053: 2048: 2043: 2038: 2033: 2028: 2023: 2018: 2013: 2008: 2002: 2000: 1996: 1995: 1993: 1992: 1987: 1985:Thorson's rule 1982: 1977: 1972: 1967: 1962: 1957: 1952: 1947: 1942: 1937: 1932: 1927: 1922: 1917: 1912: 1910:Assembly rules 1906: 1904: 1896: 1895: 1893: 1892: 1887: 1882: 1877: 1872: 1867: 1866: 1865: 1855: 1850: 1845: 1840: 1835: 1829: 1827: 1821: 1820: 1818: 1817: 1812: 1807: 1795: 1793:Patch dynamics 1790: 1788:Metapopulation 1785: 1780: 1775: 1770: 1765: 1760: 1755: 1750: 1745: 1740: 1735: 1730: 1725: 1720: 1715: 1710: 1704: 1702: 1694: 1693: 1691: 1690: 1685: 1683:Storage effect 1680: 1675: 1670: 1665: 1660: 1655: 1650: 1645: 1639: 1637: 1631: 1630: 1628: 1627: 1622: 1617: 1612: 1607: 1602: 1597: 1592: 1587: 1582: 1577: 1572: 1567: 1565:Neutral theory 1562: 1557: 1552: 1550:Native species 1543: 1538: 1533: 1528: 1523: 1518: 1513: 1508: 1503: 1497: 1495: 1491: 1490: 1488: 1487: 1482: 1481: 1480: 1475: 1465: 1460: 1455: 1450: 1445: 1440: 1435: 1430: 1425: 1423:Overpopulation 1420: 1415: 1410: 1405: 1400: 1395: 1390: 1385: 1380: 1375: 1369: 1367: 1359: 1358: 1346: 1345: 1338: 1331: 1323: 1314: 1313: 1311: 1310: 1305: 1300: 1295: 1290: 1285: 1280: 1275: 1269: 1267: 1261: 1260: 1258: 1257: 1252: 1247: 1242: 1237: 1232: 1230:Nutrient cycle 1227: 1222: 1220:Feeding frenzy 1217: 1212: 1207: 1202: 1200:Energy quality 1197: 1192: 1187: 1182: 1177: 1172: 1167: 1162: 1160:Cascade effect 1157: 1152: 1146: 1144: 1140: 1139: 1137: 1136: 1135: 1134: 1129: 1124: 1119: 1114: 1109: 1104: 1094: 1089: 1084: 1079: 1073: 1071: 1067: 1066: 1064: 1063: 1058: 1053: 1048: 1043: 1038: 1032: 1030: 1024: 1023: 1021: 1020: 1015: 1010: 1005: 1003:Microbial loop 1000: 995: 990: 985: 980: 975: 970: 968:Lithoautotroph 965: 960: 954: 952: 950:Microorganisms 946: 945: 943: 942: 937: 932: 927: 921: 919: 913: 912: 910: 909: 907:Prey switching 904: 899: 894: 889: 884: 879: 874: 869: 864: 859: 854: 849: 844: 839: 834: 829: 824: 818: 816: 810: 809: 807: 806: 801: 796: 791: 786: 784:Photosynthesis 781: 776: 771: 766: 761: 756: 751: 746: 741: 739:Chemosynthesis 736: 730: 728: 722: 721: 719: 718: 713: 708: 703: 698: 693: 688: 683: 678: 673: 668: 663: 658: 653: 648: 643: 638: 633: 631:Abiotic stress 628: 622: 620: 616: 615: 599: 598: 591: 584: 576: 570: 569: 564: 557: 556:External links 554: 552: 551: 542: 533: 517: 501: 492: 471:10.1086/283573 465:(4): 449–466. 449: 440: 431: 414: 402: 386: 369: 360: 347: 333: 331: 328: 327: 326: 318: 313: 308: 301: 298: 264: 261: 242: 239: 233: 230: 198: 195: 183: 180: 178: 175: 139: 136: 79: 76: 71: 68: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 2158: 2147: 2144: 2143: 2141: 2128: 2123: 2117: 2114: 2112: 2111:Urban ecology 2109: 2107: 2104: 2102: 2099: 2097: 2094: 2092: 2089: 2087: 2084: 2082: 2079: 2077: 2074: 2072: 2069: 2067: 2064: 2062: 2059: 2057: 2054: 2052: 2049: 2047: 2044: 2042: 2039: 2037: 2034: 2032: 2029: 2027: 2024: 2022: 2019: 2017: 2014: 2012: 2009: 2007: 2004: 2003: 2001: 1997: 1991: 1988: 1986: 1983: 1981: 1978: 1976: 1973: 1971: 1970:Kleiber's law 1968: 1966: 1963: 1961: 1958: 1956: 1953: 1951: 1948: 1946: 1943: 1941: 1938: 1936: 1933: 1931: 1928: 1926: 1923: 1921: 1918: 1916: 1913: 1911: 1908: 1907: 1905: 1903: 1897: 1891: 1888: 1886: 1883: 1881: 1878: 1876: 1873: 1871: 1868: 1864: 1861: 1860: 1859: 1856: 1854: 1851: 1849: 1846: 1844: 1841: 1839: 1836: 1834: 1831: 1830: 1828: 1826: 1822: 1816: 1813: 1811: 1808: 1806: 1804: 1800: 1796: 1794: 1791: 1789: 1786: 1784: 1781: 1779: 1776: 1774: 1771: 1769: 1766: 1764: 1761: 1759: 1756: 1754: 1751: 1749: 1746: 1744: 1743:Foster's rule 1741: 1739: 1736: 1734: 1731: 1729: 1726: 1724: 1721: 1719: 1716: 1714: 1711: 1709: 1706: 1705: 1703: 1701: 1695: 1689: 1686: 1684: 1681: 1679: 1676: 1674: 1671: 1669: 1666: 1664: 1661: 1659: 1656: 1654: 1651: 1649: 1646: 1644: 1641: 1640: 1638: 1632: 1626: 1623: 1621: 1618: 1616: 1613: 1611: 1608: 1606: 1603: 1601: 1598: 1596: 1593: 1591: 1588: 1586: 1583: 1581: 1578: 1576: 1573: 1571: 1568: 1566: 1563: 1561: 1558: 1556: 1553: 1551: 1547: 1544: 1542: 1539: 1537: 1534: 1532: 1529: 1527: 1524: 1522: 1519: 1517: 1514: 1512: 1509: 1507: 1504: 1502: 1499: 1498: 1496: 1492: 1486: 1483: 1479: 1476: 1474: 1471: 1470: 1469: 1466: 1464: 1461: 1459: 1456: 1454: 1451: 1449: 1446: 1444: 1441: 1439: 1436: 1434: 1431: 1429: 1426: 1424: 1421: 1419: 1416: 1414: 1411: 1409: 1406: 1404: 1401: 1399: 1396: 1394: 1391: 1389: 1386: 1384: 1381: 1379: 1376: 1374: 1371: 1370: 1368: 1366: 1360: 1355: 1351: 1344: 1339: 1337: 1332: 1330: 1325: 1324: 1321: 1309: 1306: 1304: 1301: 1299: 1296: 1294: 1291: 1289: 1286: 1284: 1281: 1279: 1276: 1274: 1271: 1270: 1268: 1262: 1256: 1253: 1251: 1248: 1246: 1243: 1241: 1238: 1236: 1233: 1231: 1228: 1226: 1223: 1221: 1218: 1216: 1213: 1211: 1208: 1206: 1203: 1201: 1198: 1196: 1193: 1191: 1188: 1186: 1183: 1181: 1178: 1176: 1173: 1171: 1168: 1166: 1163: 1161: 1158: 1156: 1153: 1151: 1148: 1147: 1145: 1141: 1133: 1130: 1128: 1125: 1123: 1120: 1118: 1115: 1113: 1110: 1108: 1105: 1103: 1100: 1099: 1098: 1095: 1093: 1090: 1088: 1085: 1083: 1080: 1078: 1075: 1074: 1072: 1068: 1062: 1061:Trophic level 1059: 1057: 1054: 1052: 1049: 1047: 1044: 1042: 1039: 1037: 1034: 1033: 1031: 1029: 1025: 1019: 1018:Phage ecology 1016: 1014: 1011: 1009: 1008:Microbial mat 1006: 1004: 1001: 999: 996: 994: 991: 989: 986: 984: 981: 979: 976: 974: 971: 969: 966: 964: 963:Bacteriophage 961: 959: 956: 955: 953: 951: 947: 941: 938: 936: 933: 931: 930:Decomposition 928: 926: 923: 922: 920: 918: 914: 908: 905: 903: 900: 898: 895: 893: 890: 888: 885: 883: 880: 878: 877:Mesopredators 875: 873: 870: 868: 865: 863: 860: 858: 855: 853: 850: 848: 845: 843: 840: 838: 835: 833: 830: 828: 825: 823: 822:Apex predator 820: 819: 817: 815: 811: 805: 802: 800: 797: 795: 792: 790: 787: 785: 782: 780: 777: 775: 772: 770: 767: 765: 762: 760: 757: 755: 752: 750: 747: 745: 742: 740: 737: 735: 732: 731: 729: 727: 723: 717: 714: 712: 709: 707: 704: 702: 699: 697: 694: 692: 689: 687: 684: 682: 679: 677: 674: 672: 669: 667: 664: 662: 659: 657: 656:Biotic stress 654: 652: 649: 647: 644: 642: 639: 637: 634: 632: 629: 627: 624: 623: 621: 617: 612: 608: 604: 597: 592: 590: 585: 583: 578: 577: 574: 568: 565: 563: 560: 559: 546: 537: 531: 524: 522: 515: 511: 505: 496: 488: 484: 480: 476: 472: 468: 464: 460: 453: 444: 435: 428: 424: 418: 412: 406: 400: 396: 390: 383: 379: 373: 364: 357: 351: 344: 338: 334: 324: 323: 319: 317: 314: 312: 309: 307: 304: 303: 297: 293: 290: 285: 283: 279: 273: 269: 260: 258: 252: 249: 238: 229: 226: 221: 218: 217: 212: 207: 203: 194: 190: 188: 174: 172: 167: 157: 153: 151: 150: 145: 135: 133: 129: 125: 123: 120:or increased 119: 115: 111: 109: 105: 101: 99: 95: 91: 88: 84: 75: 67: 65: 60: 58: 54: 50: 47:and maximize 46: 42: 39:within their 38: 31: 27: 23: 19: 2096:Regime shift 2081:Macroecology 1802: 1798: 1752: 1738:Edge effects 1708:Biogeography 1653:Commensalism 1501:Biodiversity 1378:Allee effect 1117:kelp forests 1070:Example webs 935:Detritivores 774:Organotrophs 754:Kinetotrophs 706:Productivity 545: 536: 504: 495: 462: 458: 452: 443: 434: 417: 405: 389: 372: 363: 355: 350: 342: 337: 320: 294: 289:matching law 286: 282:Cichlid fish 274: 270: 266: 253: 248:Matching Law 244: 235: 232:Shortcomings 224: 222: 214: 208: 204: 200: 191: 186: 185: 170: 162: 147: 143: 141: 131: 127: 126: 113: 112: 107: 103: 102: 97: 93: 92: 86: 82: 81: 73: 61: 53:proportional 25: 21: 15: 1733:Disturbance 1636:interaction 1458:Recruitment 1388:Depensation 1180:Copiotrophs 1051:Energy flow 973:Lithotrophy 917:Decomposers 897:Planktivore 872:Insectivore 862:Heterotroph 827:Bacterivore 794:Phototrophs 744:Chemotrophs 716:Restoration 666:Competition 138:Predictions 78:Assumptions 41:environment 2101:Sexecology 1678:Parasitism 1643:Antibiosis 1478:Resistance 1473:Resilience 1363:Population 1283:Camouflage 1235:Oligotroph 1150:Ascendency 1112:intertidal 1102:cold seeps 1056:Food chain 857:Herbivores 832:Carnivores 759:Mixotrophs 734:Autotrophs 613:components 330:References 62:The term " 30:population 2006:Allometry 1960:Emergence 1688:Symbiosis 1673:Mutualism 1468:Stability 1373:Abundance 1185:Dominance 1143:Processes 1132:tide pool 1028:Food webs 902:Predation 887:Omnivores 814:Consumers 769:Mycotroph 726:Producers 671:Ecosystem 636:Behaviour 37:resources 2140:Category 2061:Endolith 1990:Xerosere 1902:networks 1718:Ecocline 1264:Defense, 940:Detritus 842:Foraging 711:Resource 487:83753051 300:See also 159:Figure 1 57:foraging 2051:Ecopath 1858:Habitat 1728:Ecotype 1723:Ecotone 1700:ecology 1698:Spatial 1634:Species 1494:Species 1365:ecology 1350:Ecology 1298:Mimicry 1266:counter 1210:f-ratio 958:Archaea 646:Biomass 619:General 611:Trophic 603:Ecology 479:2460478 278:daphnia 177:Support 110:patch. 49:fitness 18:ecology 1082:Rivers 978:Marine 485:  477:  1999:Other 1900:Other 1853:Guild 1825:Niche 1077:Lakes 483:S2CID 475:JSTOR 395:Oikos 108:ideal 64:ideal 33:' 20:, an 1087:Soil 98:free 510:doi 467:doi 463:115 423:doi 378:doi 26:IFD 16:In 2142:: 1548:/ 1352:: 609:: 605:: 520:^ 481:. 473:. 461:. 259:. 128:5) 124:. 114:4) 104:3) 94:2) 83:1) 1803:K 1801:/ 1799:r 1342:e 1335:t 1328:v 595:e 588:t 581:v 512:: 489:. 469:: 429:. 425:: 384:. 380:: 24:(

Index

ecology
population
resources
environment
resource competition
fitness
proportional
foraging
ideal
scramble competition
interference competition
Nash equilibrium

Nash equilibrium
sugarbeet root aphid
Populus angustifolia
Matching Law
positive reinforcement
daphnia
Cichlid fish
matching law
Behavioural ecology
Marginal value theorem
Optimal foraging theory
Xylocopa sonorina
doi
10.1073/pnas.0710603105
Oikos
https://www.jstor.org/stable/3546218
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10905-005-8737-1

Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.

↑