Knowledge

Stereotypy (non-human)

Source 📝

87:
apparent stress or conflict. The development of the stereotypy into a habit and the difficulty of interrupting said habit explain why it is expected that the frequency of stereotypies increases with age. There is also some research that suggests that the persistence of stereotypies might be due to behavioural differences between animals with stereotypies and those without. One of the behavioural differences that has been researched is a higher resistance to extinction seen in animals that exhibit stereotypic behaviours. Resistance to extinction occurs when animals are unable to stop a learned response. Research with domesticated dogs has shown that dogs who exhibit common stereotypic behaviours have higher resistance to extinction in a simple experimental task than other dogs. This correlation suggests that stereotypic behaviours in dogs may have been reinforced in some way early in their development and have persisted because these dogs are highly resistant to extinction.
79:
establishment of stereotypies is exploratory behaviours that are directed to the outside of a cage. Research suggests that some common stereotypic behaviours seen in mice, wire-gnawing for example, may originate from such exploratory behaviours. It has been noted that when mice climb on the bars of the cage, they frequently attempt to sniff outside the cage by putting their nose between the bars of the cage. During this process, mice will occasionally bite the bars. Over time, the frequency of biting the bar increases while the frequency of sniffing decreases until the mice are left with only the wire-gnawing stereotypy. Although there is strong evidence to suggest that the development of stereotypies is influenced by exploratory behaviours, the mechanisms behind these exploratory behaviours are still unclear. They may be the result of some attractive stimuli outside of the cage or they may result from a motivation to escape the cage.
58:', as they exhibit themselves solely in animals subjected to barren environments, scheduled or restricted feedings, social deprivation and other cases of frustration, but do not arise in 'normal' animals in their natural environments. These behaviours may be maladaptive, involving self-injury or reduced reproductive success, and in laboratory animals can confound behavioural research. Stereotypical behaviours are thought to be caused ultimately by artificial environments that do not allow animals to satisfy their normal behavioural needs. Rather than refer to the behaviour as abnormal, it has been suggested that it be described as "behaviour indicative of an abnormal environment". 96: 176:, including larger and more stimulating enclosures, training, and introductions of stimuli (such as objects, sounds, or scents) to the animal's environment. The enrichment must be varied to remain effective for any length of time. Housing social animals such as primates with other members of their species is also helpful. However, once the behaviour is established, it is sometimes impossible to eliminate due to alterations in the brain. 71: 198:
could compromise the results. However, expressions of stereotypic behaviour can also present a unique opportunity for researchers. Just as human research into basal ganglia dysfunction provided insight into the development of stereotypies in animals, animal research on stereotypic behaviours may help understand the neuronal mechanisms behind many of the motor stereotypies seen in human clinical populations.
101: 100: 97: 102: 99: 132:. Up to 54% of elephants in zoos display stereotypical behaviors. Stereotypic behaviour is also common in captive giraffes; although they perform a wide range of stereotypies, they predominantly lick inanimate objects, which may be related to limitations on natural foraging and feeding behaviour. Stereotypies are well known in 188:
concern. Stereotypies are considered one of the most important indicators of long-term animal welfare problems. A prolonged display of stereotypies suggests that the welfare of the animal is in a peril state. The welfare of the mink is considered an important aspect from a management standpoint as it
82:
The continuance of a stereotypic behaviour can be explained by its impact on the basal ganglia and the establishment of a habit in its expression. Research in humans suggests that damage to the dorsal basal ganglia can make it very difficult for a person to switch between two motor behaviours, thus
197:
Extreme displays of stereotypies can be an animal welfare issue as well as a confound in behavioural research. Since much of the behavioural research done on animals requires the subject to have a certain level of normal behavioural functioning, any stereotypic behaviour exhibited by the subjects
86:
The interruption or cease of a habit is much more tedious and difficult than that of the initial behaviour. As stereotypies develop, they become more readily elicited, so much so that they are no longer just expressed during the original circumstances and may be expressed in the absence of any
117:
Many stereotypies can be induced by confinement; for example, cats pace in zoo cages. Pregnant sows whose feed is restricted bite at their stalls' bars and chew without anything in their mouths. In laboratory rats and mice, grooming is the most common activity other than sleep, and grooming
78:
The display of stereotypies is usually increased in an individual over time due to the changing motivation of the stereotypy. The establishment of a stereotypy may be due to a number of factors within a captive environment. One of the factors within a captive environment that may affect the
61:
Stereotyped behaviour can also refer to normal behaviours that show low variation. For example, mammalian chewing cycles or fish capturing prey using suction feeding. Highly stereotyped movements may be due to mechanical constraint (such as the skull of a viper or fish, in which bones are
83:
they will continuously perform the same behaviour. Similar difficulties in switching between motor behaviours have been seen in bank voles which suggests that basal ganglia dysfunction might be a factor in the development of stereotypies in rodents and other animals.
98: 476:
Würbel, Hanno; Stauffacher, Markus; von Holst, Dietrich (1996-01-12). "Stereotypies in Laboratory Mice — Quantitative and Qualitative Description of the Ontogeny of 'Wire-gnawing' and 'Jumping' in Zur:ICR and Zur:ICR nu".
27: 121:
Examples of stereotypical behaviours include pacing, rocking, swimming in circles, excessive sleeping, self-mutilation (including feather picking and excessive grooming), and mouthing cage bars.
274: 62:
mechanically linked), tight neural control (as in mammalian chewing), or both. The degree of stereotyping may vary markedly between closely related species engaging in the same behaviour.
1077:
Garner, Joseph P.; Mason, Georgia J.; Smith, Rebecca (2003). "Stereotypic route-tracing in experimentally caged songbirds correlates with general behavioural disinhibition".
853:
Bashaw, Meredith J; Tarou, Loraine R; Maki, Todd S; Maple, Terry L (2001). "A survey assessment of variables related to stereotypy in captive giraffe and okapi".
687:
Swaisgood, R.R.; Shepherdson, D.J. (2005). "Scientific approaches to enrichment and stereotypies in zoo animals: what's been done and where should we go next?".
1024:
Garner, Joseph P; Mason, Georgia J (October 2002). "Evidence for a relationship between cage stereotypies and behavioural disinhibition in laboratory rodents".
147:
issue, not only leading to facility damage from chewing, kicking, and repetitive motion, but also lead to health consequences for the animal if not addressed.
888:
Fernandez, L.T.; Bashaw, M.J.; Sartor, R.L.; Bouwens, N.R.; Maki, T.S. (2008). "Tongue twisters: feeding enrichment to reduce oral stereotypy in giraffe".
764:
Kalueff, A.V.; Wheaton, M.; Murphy, D.L. (2007). "What's wrong with my mouse model? Advances and strategies in animal modeling of anxiety and depression".
924: 54:
behaviours that are repetitive, morphologically identical and which possess no obvious goal or function. These behaviours have been defined as '
387:
Garner, JP; Mason, GJ (2002). "Evidence for a relationship between cage stereotypies and behavioural disinhibition in laboratory rodents".
207: 189:
has repercussions on the production of the animal. Inadequate welfare has been linked to poor reproductivity and poor growth rate.
952:
Vickery, Sophie S.; Mason, Georgia J. (January 2003). "Behavioral persistence in captive bears: implications for reintroduction".
1175: 352: 217: 157:
also exhibit stereotypies when they are caged. Common stereotypies in these species include head throwing and jaw clamping.
139:, usually developing as a result of being confined, particularly with insufficient exercise. They are colloquially called 50:
behaviour has several meanings, leading to ambiguity in the scientific literature. A stereotypy is a term for a group of
715: 716:"A review of behavioral factors involved in the development and continued performance of stereotypic behaviors in pigs" 353:"Stereotypies in female farm mink may be genetically transmitted with higher fertility due to effects on body weight" 74:
An exploratory sniffing behaviour that may lead to the development of the wire-gnawing stereotypy in a caged mouse
26: 935: 807: 251: 525:
Luria, A. R. (March 1965). "Two Kinds of Motor Perseveration in Massive Injury of the Frontal Lobes".
628:
Keebaugh, A; Mitchell, H; Gaval-Cruz, M; Freeman, K; Edwards, G; Weinshenker, D; Thomas, J (2011).
184:
The development and continued expression of stereotypies in captive animals can quickly become an
1180: 212: 55: 575:(2014). "Association between increased behavioral persistence and stereotypy in the pet dog". 173: 118:
stereotypies have been used to investigate several animal models of anxiety and depression.
641: 486: 164:. Common stereotypic behaviours in dogs include circling, light fixation and fly snapping. 8: 645: 490: 1151: 1104: 1059: 1006: 789: 664: 629: 610: 498: 422: 1139: 1037: 866: 400: 1185: 1143: 1051: 998: 984: 905: 870: 781: 735: 669: 602: 550: 542: 502: 414: 319:"Temperament, stereotypies and anticipatory behaviour as measures of welfare in mink" 318: 1155: 1108: 1063: 1010: 793: 614: 444: 426: 1135: 1094: 1086: 1041: 1033: 988: 980: 897: 862: 832: 773: 727: 696: 659: 649: 592: 584: 534: 494: 404: 396: 368: 364: 334: 330: 297: 289: 150: 51: 1123: 654: 588: 185: 19:
This article is about the behavior in animals. For stereotypies in humans, see
16:
Non-pathological pattern of animal behavior which displays very low variability
777: 1169: 743: 546: 506: 293: 1147: 1090: 1055: 1002: 909: 874: 785: 731: 673: 606: 554: 538: 418: 222: 140: 739: 597: 572: 111: 107: 227: 144: 30:
An elephant exhibiting stereotyped trunk swinging and rocking behaviour
20: 1099: 1046: 993: 901: 700: 409: 302: 129: 1126:; Lewis, Mark H (1999). "A Rodent Model of Spontaneous Stereotypy". 154: 35: 172:
Stereotypical behaviour can sometimes be reduced or eliminated by
70: 627: 125: 630:"PRTFDC1 Is a Genetic Modifier of HPRT-Deficiency in the Mouse" 133: 136: 887: 1121: 475: 161: 566: 564: 275:"Age and context affect the stereotypies of caged mink" 561: 852: 763: 686: 570: 443:
Davis, E.; Down, N.; Garner, J; et al. (2004).
438: 436: 350: 971:Mason, G.J. (1991). "Stereotypies and suffering". 433: 124:Stereotypies are seen in many species, including 1167: 1076: 833:"Elephant deaths at zoos reignite animal debate" 252:"Home | Clemson University, South Carolina" 713: 442: 382: 380: 378: 316: 351:Jeppesen, L.; Heller, K.; Bidsoe, M. (2004). 951: 571:Protopopova, Alexandra; Hall, Nathaniel J.; 445:"Stereotypical behavior: a LAREF discussion" 1023: 386: 375: 1098: 1045: 992: 881: 824: 663: 653: 596: 408: 301: 922: 808:"Defra Final Report on Elephant Welfare" 208:Abnormal behaviour of birds in captivity 94: 69: 65: 25: 846: 1168: 218:List of abnormal behaviours in animals 970: 830: 524: 272: 246: 244: 520: 518: 516: 346: 344: 268: 266: 264: 1122:Powell, Susan B; Newman, Howard A; 110:-induced stereotypy reminiscent of 13: 714:Lawrence, AB; Terlouw, EM (1993). 499:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1996.tb01133.x 241: 192: 106:Video of a mouse showing distinct 14: 1197: 932:University of Minnesota Extension 925:"Horse Behavior and Stable Vices" 513: 341: 317:Hansen, S.; Jeppesen, L. (2006). 261: 179: 160:Stereotypies can also be seen in 855:Applied Animal Behaviour Science 323:Applied Animal Science Behaviour 1115: 1070: 1017: 964: 945: 916: 800: 757: 707: 680: 621: 469: 369:10.1016/j.applanim.2003.11.011 335:10.1016/j.applanim.2005.10.005 310: 1: 1176:Abnormal behaviour in animals 1140:10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00303-5 1038:10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00111-0 867:10.1016/s0168-1591(01)00137-x 401:10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00111-0 234: 985:10.1016/0376-6357(91)90013-p 655:10.1371/journal.pone.0022381 589:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.04.009 167: 7: 201: 90: 10: 1202: 1026:Behavioural Brain Research 18: 1128:Physiology & Behavior 778:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.01.023 923:Christie, Julie (2008). 831:Stern, A. (2005-02-28). 357:Applied Animal Behaviour 230:(for human stereotypies) 174:environmental enrichment 294:10.1163/156853993x00029 1091:10.1006/anbe.2002.2254 732:10.2527/1993.71102815x 213:Animal welfare science 114: 75: 31: 973:Behavioural Processes 577:Behavioural Processes 105: 73: 66:Onset and persistence 29: 539:10.1093/brain/88.1.1 646:2011PLoSO...622381K 491:1996Ethol.102..371W 151:Asiatic black bears 1124:Pendergast, Jane F 273:Mason, G. (1993). 143:. They present a 115: 76: 32: 902:10.1002/zoo.20180 701:10.1002/zoo.20066 573:Wynne, Clive D.L. 452:Lab Primate Newsl 155:Malayan sun bears 103: 1193: 1160: 1159: 1119: 1113: 1112: 1102: 1079:Animal Behaviour 1074: 1068: 1067: 1049: 1021: 1015: 1014: 996: 979:(2–3): 103–115. 968: 962: 961: 949: 943: 942: 940: 934:. Archived from 929: 920: 914: 913: 885: 879: 878: 850: 844: 843: 841: 840: 828: 822: 821: 819: 817: 812: 804: 798: 797: 761: 755: 754: 752: 751: 742:. Archived from 711: 705: 704: 684: 678: 677: 667: 657: 625: 619: 618: 600: 568: 559: 558: 522: 511: 510: 473: 467: 466: 464: 463: 449: 440: 431: 430: 412: 384: 373: 372: 348: 339: 338: 314: 308: 307: 305: 279: 270: 259: 258: 256: 248: 104: 36:animal behaviour 1201: 1200: 1196: 1195: 1194: 1192: 1191: 1190: 1166: 1165: 1164: 1163: 1120: 1116: 1075: 1071: 1022: 1018: 969: 965: 950: 946: 938: 927: 921: 917: 886: 882: 851: 847: 838: 836: 829: 825: 815: 813: 810: 806: 805: 801: 766:Behav Brain Res 762: 758: 749: 747: 726:(10): 2815–25. 712: 708: 685: 681: 626: 622: 569: 562: 523: 514: 474: 470: 461: 459: 447: 441: 434: 389:Behav Brain Res 385: 376: 349: 342: 315: 311: 277: 271: 262: 254: 250: 249: 242: 237: 204: 195: 193:Animal research 182: 170: 95: 93: 68: 24: 17: 12: 11: 5: 1199: 1189: 1188: 1183: 1181:Animal welfare 1178: 1162: 1161: 1134:(2): 355–363. 1114: 1085:(4): 711–727. 1069: 1016: 963: 944: 941:on 2011-11-02. 915: 896:(3): 200–212. 880: 861:(3): 235–247. 845: 823: 799: 756: 706: 695:(6): 499–518. 679: 620: 598:2286/R.I.27039 560: 512: 485:(3): 371–385. 468: 432: 374: 363:(1): 137–143. 340: 329:(1): 172–182. 309: 288:(2): 191–229. 260: 239: 238: 236: 233: 232: 231: 225: 220: 215: 210: 203: 200: 194: 191: 186:animal welfare 181: 180:Animal welfare 178: 169: 166: 92: 89: 67: 64: 15: 9: 6: 4: 3: 2: 1198: 1187: 1184: 1182: 1179: 1177: 1174: 1173: 1171: 1157: 1153: 1149: 1145: 1141: 1137: 1133: 1129: 1125: 1118: 1110: 1106: 1101: 1096: 1092: 1088: 1084: 1080: 1073: 1065: 1061: 1057: 1053: 1048: 1043: 1039: 1035: 1031: 1027: 1020: 1012: 1008: 1004: 1000: 995: 990: 986: 982: 978: 974: 967: 959: 955: 948: 937: 933: 926: 919: 911: 907: 903: 899: 895: 891: 884: 876: 872: 868: 864: 860: 856: 849: 834: 827: 809: 803: 795: 791: 787: 783: 779: 775: 771: 767: 760: 746:on 2008-07-26 745: 741: 737: 733: 729: 725: 721: 717: 710: 702: 698: 694: 690: 683: 675: 671: 666: 661: 656: 651: 647: 643: 640:(7): e22381. 639: 635: 631: 624: 616: 612: 608: 604: 599: 594: 590: 586: 582: 578: 574: 567: 565: 556: 552: 548: 544: 540: 536: 532: 528: 521: 519: 517: 508: 504: 500: 496: 492: 488: 484: 480: 472: 457: 453: 446: 439: 437: 428: 424: 420: 416: 411: 406: 402: 398: 394: 390: 383: 381: 379: 370: 366: 362: 358: 354: 347: 345: 336: 332: 328: 324: 320: 313: 304: 299: 295: 291: 287: 283: 276: 269: 267: 265: 253: 247: 245: 240: 229: 226: 224: 221: 219: 216: 214: 211: 209: 206: 205: 199: 190: 187: 177: 175: 165: 163: 158: 156: 152: 148: 146: 142: 138: 135: 131: 128:, birds, and 127: 122: 119: 113: 109: 88: 84: 80: 72: 63: 59: 57: 53: 49: 45: 41: 37: 28: 22: 1131: 1127: 1117: 1082: 1078: 1072: 1032:(1): 83–92. 1029: 1025: 1019: 976: 972: 966: 957: 953: 947: 936:the original 931: 918: 893: 889: 883: 858: 854: 848: 837:. Retrieved 826: 814:. Retrieved 802: 769: 765: 759: 748:. Retrieved 744:the original 723: 719: 709: 692: 688: 682: 637: 633: 623: 580: 576: 530: 526: 482: 478: 471: 460:. Retrieved 455: 451: 395:(1): 83–92. 392: 388: 360: 356: 326: 322: 312: 285: 281: 223:Stable vices 196: 183: 171: 159: 149: 141:stable vices 123: 120: 116: 85: 81: 77: 60: 47: 43: 39: 33: 890:Zoo Biology 816:16 November 772:(1): 1–18. 533:(1): 1–10. 112:nail biting 108:amphetamine 48:stereotyped 44:stereotypic 1170:Categories 1100:10214/4714 1047:10214/4711 994:10214/4670 839:2006-05-30 750:2011-05-13 720:J Anim Sci 462:2009-12-21 410:10214/4711 303:10214/4679 235:References 228:Stereotypy 145:management 130:carnivores 52:phenotypic 40:stereotypy 21:Stereotypy 835:. Reuters 583:: 77–81. 547:0006-8950 507:1439-0310 282:Behaviour 168:Solutions 1186:Ethology 1156:36677716 1148:10336165 1109:53153853 1064:12731260 1056:12385793 1011:43817366 1003:24923970 960:: 35–43. 910:19360618 875:11376840 794:16158758 786:17306892 689:Zoo Biol 674:21818316 634:PLOS ONE 615:10034066 607:24814910 555:14280275 479:Ethology 458:(4): 3–4 427:12731260 419:12385793 202:See also 126:primates 91:Examples 56:abnormal 740:8226385 665:3144895 642:Bibcode 487:Bibcode 134:stabled 1154:  1146:  1107:  1062:  1054:  1009:  1001:  908:  873:  792:  784:  738:  672:  662:  613:  605:  553:  545:  505:  425:  417:  137:horses 1152:S2CID 1105:S2CID 1060:S2CID 1007:S2CID 954:Ursus 939:(PDF) 928:(PDF) 811:(PDF) 790:S2CID 611:S2CID 527:Brain 448:(PDF) 423:S2CID 278:(PDF) 255:(PDF) 1144:PMID 1052:PMID 999:PMID 906:PMID 871:PMID 818:2011 782:PMID 736:PMID 670:PMID 603:PMID 551:PMID 543:ISSN 503:ISSN 415:PMID 162:dogs 153:and 1136:doi 1095:hdl 1087:doi 1042:hdl 1034:doi 1030:136 989:hdl 981:doi 898:doi 863:doi 774:doi 770:179 728:doi 697:doi 660:PMC 650:doi 593:hdl 585:doi 581:106 535:doi 495:doi 483:102 405:hdl 397:doi 393:136 365:doi 331:doi 298:hdl 290:doi 286:127 46:or 34:In 1172:: 1150:. 1142:. 1132:66 1130:. 1103:. 1093:. 1083:66 1081:. 1058:. 1050:. 1040:. 1028:. 1005:. 997:. 987:. 977:25 975:. 958:14 956:. 930:. 904:. 894:27 892:. 869:. 859:73 857:. 788:. 780:. 768:. 734:. 724:71 722:. 718:. 693:24 691:. 668:. 658:. 648:. 636:. 632:. 609:. 601:. 591:. 579:. 563:^ 549:. 541:. 531:88 529:. 515:^ 501:. 493:. 481:. 456:34 454:. 450:. 435:^ 421:. 413:. 403:. 391:. 377:^ 361:86 359:. 355:. 343:^ 327:99 325:. 321:. 296:. 284:. 280:. 263:^ 243:^ 42:, 38:, 1158:. 1138:: 1111:. 1097:: 1089:: 1066:. 1044:: 1036:: 1013:. 991:: 983:: 912:. 900:: 877:. 865:: 842:. 820:. 796:. 776:: 753:. 730:: 703:. 699:: 676:. 652:: 644:: 638:6 617:. 595:: 587:: 557:. 537:: 509:. 497:: 489:: 465:. 429:. 407:: 399:: 371:. 367:: 337:. 333:: 306:. 300:: 292:: 257:. 23:.

Index

Stereotypy

animal behaviour
phenotypic
abnormal

amphetamine
nail biting
primates
carnivores
stabled
horses
stable vices
management
Asiatic black bears
Malayan sun bears
dogs
environmental enrichment
animal welfare
Abnormal behaviour of birds in captivity
Animal welfare science
List of abnormal behaviours in animals
Stable vices
Stereotypy


"Home | Clemson University, South Carolina"


Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License. Additional terms may apply.